Advertisement

Value and Limitations of Infant Tests: An Overview

  • Marjorie P. Honzik

Abstract

The development of intelligence during infancy is impressive and measurable. The anthropometrist measures growth in head size, which reflects the growth of the brain. The neuropathologist measures cerebral DNA to estimate cell number and possible damage from malnutrition or other causes (Winick, 1970). The psychologist measures behavioral change by means of careful observations of responses to specific tasks. In this chapter we shall review critically infant tests and their contribution to the understanding of mental growth in the first months of life.

Keywords

Test Score Test Item Mental Test Maternal Deprivation Infant Test 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. D., 1962, The effects of maternal deprivation: A review of findings and controversy in the context of research strategy, in “Deprivation of Maternal Care: A Reassessment of Its Effects,” World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland, p. 97.Google Scholar
  2. Baldwin, A. L., Kalhorn, J., and Breese, F. H., 1945, Patterns of parent behavior, Psychological Monograph, 58(Whole No. 268).Google Scholar
  3. Bayley, N., 1933, “The California First Year Mental Scale,” University of California Press, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  4. Bayley, N., 1949, Consistency and variability in the growth of intelligence from birth to eighteen years, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 75:165.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bayley, N., 1954, Some increasing parent-child similarities during the growth of children, Journal of Educational Psychology, 45:1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bayley, N., 1968, Behavioral correlates of mental growth: Birth to 36 years, American Psychologist, 23:1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bayley, N., 1969, “Bayley Scales of Infant Development,” Psychological Corp., New York.Google Scholar
  8. Bayley, N., Rhodes, L., Gooch, B., and Marcus, M., 1971, Environmental factors in the development of institutionalized children, in “Exceptional Infant: Studies in Abnormalities,” Vol. 2, J. Hellmuth (ed.), Brunner/Mazel, New York.Google Scholar
  9. Bayley, N. and Schaefer, E. S., 1964, Correlations of maternal and child behaviors with the development of mental abilities: Data from the Berkeley Growth Study, Monographs for the Society for Research and Child Development, 29 (6, Whole No. 97).Google Scholar
  10. Becewith, L., 1971, Relationship between attributes of mothers and their infants’ IQ scores, Child Development, 42:1083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bernstein, B., 1961, Social class and linguistic development: A theory of social learning, in “Economy, Education and Society,” A. H. Halsey, J. Floud, and C. A. Anderson (eds.), Free Press, New York.Google Scholar
  12. Bierman, J. M., Connor, A., Vaage, M., and Honzik, M. P., 1964, Pediatricians’ assessments of the intelligence of two-year-olds and their mental test scores, Pediatrics, 34:680.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bing, E., 1963, Effect of child-rearing practices on development of differential cognitive abilities, Child Development, 34:631.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Bowlby, J., 1952, Maternal care and mental health, Monograph Series, No. 2, (2nd ed.), World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland.Google Scholar
  15. Broman, S. H., Nichols, P. L., and Kennedy, W. A., 1975, “Preschool IQ: Prenatal and Early Developmental Correlates,” Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  16. Brucefors, A., 1972, Trends in development of abilities, paper given at Réunion de coordination des recherches sur la croissance et le développement de l’enfant normal, Institute of Child Health, London.Google Scholar
  17. Brucefors, A., Johannesson, I., Karlberg, P., Klackenberg-Larsson, I., Lichenstein, H., and Svenberg, I., 1974, Trends in development of abilities related to somatic growth, Human Development, 17:152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Brunet, O., and Lezine, P. U. F., 1951, “Le développement psychologique de la première enfance,” Éditions Scientifiques et Psychotechniques, Issy-les-Moulineaux.Google Scholar
  19. Cameron, J., Llvson, N., and Bayley, N., 1967, Infant vocalizations and their relationship to mature intelligence, Science, 157:331.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Casler, L., 1961, Maternal deprivation: A critical review of the literature, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 26(2:Whole No. 80).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Casler, L., 1965, The effects of extra tactile stimulation on a group of institutionalized infants, Genetic Psychology Monographs, 71:137.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Cattell, P., 1940, “The Measurement of Intelligence in Infants and Young Children,” Science Press, New York. Reprinted by Psychological Corp., 1960.Google Scholar
  23. Chase, H. P., and Martin, H. P., 1969, Undernutrition and child development, paper read before the Conference on Neuropsychological Methods for the Assessment of Impaired Brain Functioning in the Malnourished Child, Palo Alto, California.Google Scholar
  24. Cravioto, J., and Delicardie, E., 1970, Mental performance in school age children, American Journal of Diseases of Children, 120:404.Google Scholar
  25. Cravioto, J., and Robles, B., 1965, Evolution of adaptive and motor behavior during rehabilitation from kwashiorkor, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 35:449.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Dennis, W., and Najarian, P., 1957, Infant development under environmental handicap, Psychological Monographs, 71(7, Whole No. 436).Google Scholar
  27. Dodgson, M. C. H., 1962, “The Growing Brain: An Essay in Developmental Neurology,” Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  28. Erickson, M. T., Johnson, N. M., and Campbell, F. A., 1970, Relationships among scores on infant tests for children with developmental problems, American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 75:102.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., and Jarvik, L. F., 1963, Genetics and intelligence: A review, Science, 142:1477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Escalona, S., 1968, “The Roots of Individuality: Normal Patterns of Individuality,” Aldine Publishing Company, Chicago.Google Scholar
  31. Fantz, R. L., And Nevis, S., 1967, The predictive value of changes in visual preferences in early infancy, in “Exceptional Infant: The Normal Infant,” Vol. 1, J. Hellmuth (ed.), Brunner/Mazel, New York.Google Scholar
  32. Frankenburg, W. K., Camp, B. W., and van Natta, P. A., 1971, Validity of the Denver Developmental Screening Test, Child Development, 42:475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Frankenburg, W. K., and Dodds, J. B., 1967, The Denver Developmental. Screening Test, Journal of Pediatrics, 71:181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Freedman, D. G., and Keller, B., 1963, Inheritance of behavior in infants, Science, 140:196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gesell, A., and Amatruda, C., 1941, “Developmental Diagnosis,” Paul B. Hoeber, New York.Google Scholar
  36. Gesell, A., Halverson, H. M., ILG, F. L., Thompson, H., Castner, B. M., Ames, L. B., and Amatruda, C. S., 1940, “The First Five Years of Life,” Harper, New York.Google Scholar
  37. Gesell, A., and Thompson, H., 1934, “Infant Behavior: Its Genesis and Growth,” McGraw-Hill, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Goffeney, B., Henderson, N. B., and Butler, B. V., 1971, Negro-White, male-female 8-month developmental scores compared with 7-year WISC and Bender Test Scores, Child Development, 42:595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Griffiths, R., 1954, “The Abilities of Babies,” McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  40. Harlow, H. F., 1958, The nature of love, American Psychologist, 13:673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hindley, C. B., 1960, The Griffiths Scale of Infant Development: Scores and predictions from 3 to 18 months, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1:99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hindley, C. B., 1962, Social class influences on the development of ability in the first five years, Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Child and Education, 3:29.Google Scholar
  43. Hindley, C. B., 1965, Stability and change in abilities up to five years: Group trends, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6:85.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Honzik, M. P., 1938, The constancy of mental test performance during the preschool period, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 52:285.Google Scholar
  45. Honzik, M. P., 1957, Developmental studies of parent-child resemblance in intelligence, Child Development, 28:215.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Honzix, M. P., 1963, A sex difference in the age of onset of the parent-child resemblance in intelligence, Journal of Educational Psychology, 54:231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Honzik, M. P., 1966, The environment and mental growth from 21 months to 30 years, XVIII International Congress of Psychology Proceedings, 11:28.Google Scholar
  48. Honzik, M. P., 1967, Environmental correlates of mental growth: Prediction from the family setting at 21 months, Child Development, 38:337.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Honzik, M. P., 1972, Intellectual abilities at age 40 in relation to the early family environment, in “Determinants of Behavioral Development,” F. J. Monks, W. W. Hartup, and J. de Wit (eds.), Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  50. Honzik, M. P., Hutchings, J. J., and Burnip, S. R. 1965, Birth record assessments and test performance at eight months, American Journal of Diseases of Children, 109:416.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Honzik, M. P., Macfarlane, J. W., and Allen, L., 1948, Stability of mental test performance between 2 and 18 years, Journal of Experimental Education, 17:309.Google Scholar
  52. Illingworth, R. S., 1960, “The Development of the Infant and Young Child: Normal and Abnormal,” E. & S. Livingstone, Ltd., Edinburgh and London.Google Scholar
  53. Ireton, H., Thwing, E., and Gravem, H., 1970, Infant mental development and neurological status, family socioeconomic status, and intelligence at age four, Child Development, 41:937.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Kagan, J., 1971, “Change and Continuity in Infancy,” Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  55. Kagan, J., and Klein, R. E., 1973, Cross-cultural perspectives in early development, American Psychologist, 28:947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Klackenberg-Larsson, I., and Stensson, J., 1968, Data on the mental development during the first five years, in “The Development of Children in a Swedish Urban Community: A Prospective Longitudinal Study,” Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, Supplement 187, IV, Almqvist and Wiksell, Stockholm.Google Scholar
  57. Knobloch, H., and Pasamanick, B., 1960, An evaluation of the consistency and predictive value of the 40-week Gesell Developmental Schedule, paper presented at the Regional Research Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, Iowa City.Google Scholar
  58. Lewis, M., 1971, Individual differences in the measurement of early cognitive growth, in “Exceptional Infant: Studies in Abnormalities,” Vol. 2, J. Hellmuth (ed.), Brunner/ Mazel, New York.Google Scholar
  59. Lewis, M., 1973, Intelligence tests: Their use and misuse, Human Development, 16:1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Lewis, M., and Mcgurk, H., 1972, Evaluation of infant intelligence, Science, 178:1174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Macrae, J. M., 1955, Retests of children given mental tests as infants, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 87:111.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Mccall, R. B., Hogarty, P. S., and Hurlburt, N., 1972, Transitions in infant sensori-motor development and the prediction of childhood IQ, American Psychologist, 27:728.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Mönckeberg, F., 1968, Effect of early marasmic malnutrition on subsequent physical and psychological development, in “Malnutrition, Learning and Behavior,” N. E. Scrimshaw and J. E. Gordon (eds.), MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, p. 269.Google Scholar
  64. Moore, T., 1968, Language and intelligence: A longitudinal study of the first eight years, Part II: Environmental correlates of mental growth, Human Development, 11:1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Nelson, V. L., and Richards, T. W., 1939, Studies in mental development, III: Performance of twelve-month-old children on the Gesell Schedule and its predictive value for mental status at two and three years, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 54:181.Google Scholar
  66. Nichols, P. L., and Badman, S. H., 1974, Familial resemblance in infant mental development, Developmental Psychology, 10:442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Pollitr, E., and Grandee, D., 1967, Mental and motor development of Peruvian children treated for severe malnutrition, Review of Interamericana Psicologia, 1:93.Google Scholar
  68. Ramey, C. T., Campbell, F. A., and Nicholson, J. E., 1973, The predictive power of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development and the Stanford—Binet Intelligence Test in a relatively constant environment, Child Development, 44:790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rheingold, H. L., and Bayley, N., 1959, The later effects of an experimental modification of mothering, Child Development, 31:363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Skodak, M., and Skeels, H. M., 1949, A final follow-up study of 100 adopted children, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 75:85.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Ucxo, L. E., 1965, A comparative study of asphyxiated and non-asphyxiated boys from birth to five years, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 7:643.Google Scholar
  72. Uzgiris, I. C., and Hunt, J. McV., 1966, An instrument for assessing infant psychological development, mimeographed paper, Psychological Development Laboratories, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
  73. Werner, E. E., and Bayley, N., 1966, The reliability of Bayleÿ s revised scale of mental and motor development during the first year of life, Child Development, 37:39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Werner, E. E., Bierman, J. M., and French, F. E., 1971, “The Children of Kauai: A Longitudinal Study from the Prenatal Period to Age 10,” University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu.Google Scholar
  75. Werner, E. E., Honzik, M. P., and Smith, R. S., 1968, Prediction of intelligence and achievement at 10 years from 20-month pediatric and psychologic examinations, Child Development, 39:1063.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Willerman, L., Broman, S. H., and Fiedler, M., 1970, Infant development, preschool IQ, and social class, Child Development, 41:69.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wilson, R., 1972, Twins: Early mental development, Science, 175:914.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Wilson, R. S., and Harpring, E. B., 1972, Mental and motor development in infant twins, Developmental Psychology, 7:277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Winicx, M., 1970, Fetal malnutrition and growth processes, Hospital Practice, 5:33.Google Scholar
  80. Yarrow, L. J., 1961, Maternal deprivation: Toward an empirical and conceptual reevaluation, Psychological Bulletin, 58:459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Yarrow, L. J., 1963, Research in dimensions of early maternal care, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 9:101.Google Scholar
  82. Yarrow, L. J., Goodwin, M. S., Manheimer, H., and Milowe, I. D., 1973, Infancy experiences and cognitive and personality development at ten years, in “The Competent Infant: Research and Commentary,” L. J. Stone, H. T. Smith, and L. B. Murphy (eds.), Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  83. Yarrow, L. J., Rubenstein, J. L., Pedersen, F. A., and Jankowski, J. J., 1972, Dimensions of early stimulation and their differential effects on infant development, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18:205.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marjorie P. Honzik
    • 1
  1. 1.University of California at BerkeleyUSA

Personalised recommendations