Advertisement

Social Class and Infant Intelligence

  • Mark Golden
  • Beverly Birns

Abstract

There is generally widespread agreement that social class differences in intellectual development are present in older preschool children, as reflected in their performance on standard intelligence tests beginning during the third year of life, and later in their academic achievement in school. However, there is disagreement about whether such differences are present in infancy. There has also been a great deal of controversy about how social class differences in intellectual development can best be explained or interpreted, as well as strong disagreement about what can or should be done about such differences.

Keywords

Social Class Intellectual Performance Mark Golden Social Class Difference Standard Intelligence Test 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ausubel, D. P., 1964, How reversible are the cognitive and motivational effects of cultural deprivation? Urban Education, Summer: 16.Google Scholar
  2. Bayley, N., 1965, Comparisons of mental and motor test scores for ages 1–15 months by sex, birth order, race, geographic location, and education of parents, Child Development, 36: 379.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bee, H. L., van Egeren, L. F., Streissguth, A. P., Nyman, B. A., and Leckie, M. S., 1969, Social class differences in maternal teaching strategies and speech patterns, Developmental Psychology, 1: 726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bernstein, B., 1960, Language and social class, British Journal of Sociology, 11: 271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Binet, A., 1909, “Les idées modernes sur les enfants,” Ernest Flammarion, Paris.Google Scholar
  6. Birns, B., and Golden, M., 1972, Prediction of intellectual performance at three years on the basis of infant tests and personality measures, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18: 53.Google Scholar
  7. Birns, B., and Golden, M., March 1973, The interaction of social class and sex on intelligence, language, personality, and the mother-child relationship, paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  8. Blank, M., 1964, Some maternal influences on infants’ rates of sensorimotor development, Journal of Child Psychiatry, 3: 668.Google Scholar
  9. Bowlby, J., 1951, “Maternal Care and Mental Health,” World Health Organization, Geneva.Google Scholar
  10. Bronfenbrenner, U., 1974, “A Report on Longitudinal Evaluations of Preschool Programs,” Vol. 2, “Is Early Intervention Effective?” Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Publications No. ( OHD ), 74–25.Google Scholar
  11. Caldwell, B. M., 1970, The rationale for early intervention, Exceptional Children, 36: 717.Google Scholar
  12. Clarke-Stewart, K. A., 1973, Interactions between mothers and their young children: Characteristics and consequences, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38: (Serial No. 153).Google Scholar
  13. Corman, H., and Escalona, S. K., 1969, Stages of sensorimotor development: A replication study, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 15: 351.Google Scholar
  14. Costello, J., August 1970, Review and summary of “A National Survey of the Parent-Child Center Program,” prepared for the Office of Child Development, U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.Google Scholar
  15. Denenberg, V. H. (ed.), 1970, “Education of the Infant and Young Child,” Academic Press, New York and London.Google Scholar
  16. Dennis, W., 1973, “Children of the Creche,” Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
  17. Deutsch, M., 1964, Facilitating development in the preschool child: Social and psychological perspectives, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 10: 249.Google Scholar
  18. Engelmann, S., 1970, The effectiveness of direct instruction on IQ performance and achievement in reading and arithmetic, in “Disadvantaged Child,” Vol. 3, J. Hellmuth (ed.), Brunner/hazel, New York.Google Scholar
  19. Fowler, W., 1962, Cognitive learning in infancy and early childhood, Psychological Bulletin, 59: 116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Geismar, L. L., and la Sorte, M. A., 1964, “Understanding the Multi-Problem Family,” Association Press, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Goddard, H. H., 1920, “Human Efficiency and Levels of Intelligence,” Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  22. Golden, M., and Birns, B., 1968, Social class and cognitive development in infancy, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 14: 139.Google Scholar
  23. Golden, M., Bnuvs, B., Bridger, W. H., and Moss, A., 1971, Social class differentiation in cognitive development among black preschool children, Child Development, 42: 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Golden, M., Bridger, W. H., and Monrare, A., 1974, Social class differences in the ability of young children to use verbal information to facilitate learning, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44: 86.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gordon, I. J., 1973, A home learning center approach to early stimulation, in “Revisiting Early Childhood Education,” J. L. Frost (ed.), Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  26. Heber, R., Garber, H., Harrington, C. H., and Falender, C., 1972, Rehabilitation of families at risk for mental retardation, Progress Report.Google Scholar
  27. Hess, R. D., and Shipman, V., 1965, Early experience and the socialization of cognitive modes in children, Child Development, 36: 869.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hess, R. D., and Shipman, V. C., 1967, Cognitive elements in maternal behavior, in “Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology,” J. P. Hill (ed.), University of Minnesota Press, 1: 58.Google Scholar
  29. Hindley, C. B., 1962, Social class influences on the development of ability in the first five years, in “Child and Education: Proceedings of the XIV International Congress of Applied PsychologyMunksgaard, Copenhagen, 3: 29.Google Scholar
  30. Hollingshead, A. B., 1957, “Two-Factor Index of Social Position,” published by the author, New Haven, Connecticut.Google Scholar
  31. Hunt, J. Mcv., 1961, “Intelligence and Experience,” Ronald Press, New York.Google Scholar
  32. Hunt, J. Mcv., 1969, “The Challenge of Incompetence and Poverty,” University of Illinois Press, Urbana.Google Scholar
  33. Jensen, A. R., 1969, How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review, 39: 1.Google Scholar
  34. Kagan, J., 1971, “Change and Continuity in Infancy,” Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  35. Kagan, J., and Klein, R. E., 1973, Cross-cultural perspectives on early development, American Psychologist, 28: 947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kamin, L. J., 1974, “The Science and Politics of I. Q.,” Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  37. Karnes, M. B., Teska, J. A., Hodgins, A. S., and Badger, E. D., 1970, Educational intervention at home by mothers of disadvantaged infants, Child Development, 41: 925.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Katz, P. A., 1963, Effects of labels on children’s perception and discrimination learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66: 423.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Koltsova, M. M., 1960, “The Formation of Higher Nervous Activity of the Child,” Veb Verland, Volk und Gesundheit, Berlin.Google Scholar
  40. Leacock, E. B. (ed.), 1971, “The Culture of Poverty: A Critique,” Simon and Schuster, New York.Google Scholar
  41. Levenstein, P., in press, The mother-child home program, in “The Preschool in Action,” R. K. Parker (ed.) (2nd ed.), Allyn and Bacon, Boston.Google Scholar
  42. Lewis, M., 1969, A developmental study of information processing within the first three years of life: Response decrement to a redundant signal, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 34: (Serial No. 133).Google Scholar
  43. Lewis, M., and Freedle, R., 1973, The mother-infant dyad, in “Communication and Affect: Language and Thought,” P. Pliner, L. Kranes, and T. Alloway (eds.), Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  44. Lewis, M., and Mcgurx, H., 1973, Infant intelligence scores… True or False? in “Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development,” S. Chess and A. Thomas (eds.), Brunner/Mazel, New York.Google Scholar
  45. Lewis, M., and Wilson, C. D., 1972, Infant development in lower-class American families, Human Development, 15, 112.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lewis, O., 1966, The culture of poverty, Scientific American, 215: 19.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Luria, A. R., 1961, “The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior,” Liveright, New York.Google Scholar
  48. Madden, J., Levenstein, P., and Levenstein, S., in press, Longitudinal IQ outcomes of the mother—child home program, 1967–1973, Child Development.Google Scholar
  49. Malone, C. A., 1963, Some observations on children of disorganized families and problems of acting out, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 2: 22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mccall, R. B., Hogarty, P. S., and Hurlburt, N., 1972, Transistors in infant sensorimotor development and the prediction of childhood IQ, American Psychologist, 27: 728.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mccall, R. B., Appelbaum, M. I., and Hogarty, P. S., 1973, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38:(Serial No. 150).Google Scholar
  52. Messer, S. G., and Lewis, M., 1972, Social class and sex differences in the attachment and play behavior of the year old infant, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18: 295.Google Scholar
  53. Miller, S. M., 1964, The American lower classes: A typological approach, in “Mental Health of the Poor,” F. Riessman, J. Cohen, and A. Pearl (eds.), Free Press, New York.Google Scholar
  54. Painter, G., 1969, The effect of a structured tutorial program on the cognitive and language development of culturally disadvantaged infants, Merrill—Palmer Quarterly, 15: 279.Google Scholar
  55. Palmer, F. H., 1972, Minimal intervention at age two and three and subsequent intellective changes, in “The Preschool in Action,” R. K. Parker (ed.), Allyn and Bacon, Boston.Google Scholar
  56. Pasamanick, B., and Knobloch, H., 1961, Epidemiological studies on the complications of pregnancy and the birth process, in “Prevention of Mental Disorders in Children,” G. Caplan (ed.), Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  57. Pavenstedt, E., 1965, A comparison of the child-rearing environment of the upper-lower and very low-lower class families, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 35: 89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Piaget, J., 1952, “The Origins of Intelligence in Children,” Margaret Cook (trans.), International University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  59. Rossman, E., Golden, M., Birns, B., Moss, A., and Montare, A., March 1973, Mother—child interaction, IQ, and social class, paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  60. Schaefer, E. S., 1970, Need for early and continuing education, in “Education of the Infant and Young Child,” V. H. Denenberg (ed.), Academic Press, New York and London.Google Scholar
  61. Schaefer, E. S., and Aaronson, M., 1972, Infant education research project: Implementation and implication of the home-tutoring program, in “The Preschool in Action,” R. K. Parker (ed.), Allyn and Bacon, Boston.Google Scholar
  62. Skeels, H. M., 1966, Adult status of children with contrasting early life experiences, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 31: (Serial No. 105).Google Scholar
  63. Terman, L. M., 1916, “The Measurement of Intelligence,” Houghton-Mifflin, Boston.Google Scholar
  64. Terman, L. M., and Merrill, M. A., 1937, “Measuring Intelligence,” Houghton-Mifflin, Boston.Google Scholar
  65. Tulkin, S. R., 1972, An analysis of the concept of cultural deprivation, Developmental Psychology, 6: 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tulkin, S. R., 1973, Social class differences in infants’ reactions to mother’s and stranger’s voices, Developmental Psychology, 8: 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Tulkin, S. R., and Kagan, J., 1972, Mother—child interaction in the first year of life, Child Development, 43: 31.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Uzgiris, I. C., and Hum, J. McV., 1966, An instrument for assessing infant psychological development, Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
  69. Wachs, T. D., Uzcnus, I. C., and Hunt, J. McV., 1971, Cognitive development in infants of different age levels and from different environmental backgrounds: An exploratory investigation, Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 17: 283.Google Scholar
  70. Wargo, M. J., Campeau, P. L., Tallmadge, G. K., 1961, “Further Examination of Exemplary Programs for Educationally Disadvantaged Children,” American Institute for Research in Behavioral Sciences, Palo Alto, California.Google Scholar
  71. White, B., 1975, Critical influences on the development of competence, Merrill Palmer Quarterly.Google Scholar
  72. Yarrow, L., 1961, Maternal deprivation: Toward an empirical and conceptual re-evaluation, Psychological Bulletin, 58; 459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Yarrow, L. J., Rubenstein, J. L., Pedersen, F. A., and Jankowski, J. J., 1973, Dimensions of early stimulation and their differential effects on infant development, in “Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development,” S. Chess and A. Thomas (eds.), Brunner/Mazel, New York.Google Scholar
  74. Yerkes, R. M., and Foster, J. C., 1923, “A Point Scale for Measuring Mental Ability,” Warwick and York, Baltimore.Google Scholar
  75. Zigler, E., and Butrerfield, E. C., 1968, Motivational aspects of changes in IQ test performance of culturally deprived nursery school children, Child Development, 39: 1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark Golden
    • 1
  • Beverly Birns
    • 1
  1. 1.State University of New York at Stony BrookUSA

Personalised recommendations