Advertisement

Brain/Cognition Relationships: Interpreting the Effects of Focal Brain Damage

  • Rita Sloan Berndt

Abstract

The normal human brain carries out a large number of complicated cognitive activities. This volume discusses, from a number of different perspectives, ‘exceptions’ to the normal state. That is, it considers a variety of ways in which cognitive activity can be affected by interruption of the maturational unfolding of the brain and by the physiological changes that accompany aging and disease. An important, though often neglected, requirement of research addressing these ‘exceptions’ is the understanding of the normal, mature relationship between the brain and cognitive activity. There must exist a “paradigm case” toward which the system evolves during development and from which it departs in conditions of pathology—a normal baseline against which immature and pathological conditions can be compared. There are a variety of methodologies available that have provided information about this paradigm case, i.e., about normal brain/cognition relationships.

Keywords

Left Hemisphere Cognitive Activity Focal Brain Positron Emission Tomog Focal Brain Lesion 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alexander MP, Naeser MA, and Palumbo C (1990): Broca’s area aphasias: Aphasia after lesions including the frontal operculum. Neurology 40:353–362Google Scholar
  2. Benton A (1985): Visuoperceptual, visuospatial and visuoconstructive disorders. In: Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol 2., Heilman K, Valenstein E, eds. New York: OxfordGoogle Scholar
  3. Berndt RS (1987): Symptom co-occurrence and dissociation in the interpretation of agrammatism. In: The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Language, Coltheart M, Sartori G, Job R, eds. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  4. Berndt RS (1989): Repetition in aphasia: Implications for models of language processing. In: Handbook of Neuropsychology, Boller R, Grafman J, eds. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science PublishersGoogle Scholar
  5. Berndt RS (1991): Sentence processing in aphasia. In: Acquired Aphasia, 2nd Edition, Sarno MT, ed. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  6. Berndt RS, and Caramazza A (1980): A redefinition of the syndrome of Broca’s aphasia: Implications for a neuropsychological model of language. Applied Psycholinguistics 1:225–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Berndt RS, and Caramazza A (1981): Syntactic deficits in aphasia. In: Acquired Aphasia, 1st Edition, Sarno MT, ed. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Berndt RS, Mitchum CC, and Price TR (1991): Short-term memory and sentence comprehension: An investigation of a patient with crossed aphasia. Brain 114:263–280Google Scholar
  9. Blumstein SE, Goodglass H and Tartter V (1975): The reliability of ear advantage in dichotic listening. Brain and Language 2:226–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Broca PP (1861): Remarques sur le siege de la faculté du langage articule, suives D’une observation D’aphemie. Bull Soc Anat 2:330–357Google Scholar
  11. Butterworth B, Howard D (1987): Paragrammatisms. Cognition 26:1–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caramazza A (1986): On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies. Brain and Cognition 5:41–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chiarello C (1988): Right Hemisphere Contributions to Lexical Semantics. Berlin: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Corballis MC, Sergent J (1988): Imagery in a commissurotomized patient. Neuropsychologia 26:13–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen S E (1990): At-tentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. Science 248:1556–1559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Damasio H, Damasio A (1980): The anatomical basis of conduction aphasia. Brain 103:337–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Damasio H, Damasio AR (1989): Lesion Analysis in Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  18. Ehrlichman H, Barrett J (1983): Right hemisphere specialization for mental imagery: A review of the evidence. Brain and Cognition 2:55–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Farah MJ (1984): The neurological basis of mental imagery: a componential analysis. Cognition 18:245–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Farah MJ (1986): The laterality of mental image generation: A test with normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 24:541–551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Farah MJ, Gazzaniga MS, Holtzman J, Kosslyn SM (1985): A left hemisphere basis for mental imagery? Neuropsychologia 23:115–118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gates A, Bradshaw J (1977): The role of the cerebral hemispheres in music. Brain and Language 4:403–431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Geschwind N (1965): Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain 88: 237–294, 585-644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldman-Rakic PS (1988): Topography of cognition: Parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 11: 137–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Heilman KM, Rothi L, Kertesz A (1983): Localization of apraxia-producing lesions. In: Localization in Neuropsychology, Kertesz A, ed. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  26. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Valenstein E (1985): Emotional disorders associated with neurological disease. In: Clinical Neuropsychology, 2nd Edition, Heilman KM, Valenstein E, eds. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  27. Kandel ER, Schwartz JH eds. (1985): Principles of Neural Science, 2nd Edition. New York: Elsevier North HollandGoogle Scholar
  28. Kertesz A (1983): Localization of lesions in Wernicke’s Aphasia. In: Localization in Neuropsychology, Kertesz A, ed. Orlando, FL: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  29. Kosslyn SM (1987): Seeing and imagining in the two cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychological Review 94:148–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kosslyn SM, Berndt RS, Doyle TS (1985): Imagery and language processing. In: Attention and Performance XI, Posner MI, Marin OSM, eds. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  31. Kosslyn SM, Holtzman J, Farah MJ, Gazzaniga MS (1985): A computational analysis of mental image generation: Evidence from functional dissociations in split brain patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114:311–341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kushner M, Reivich M, Fieschi C, Silver F, Chawluk J, Rosen M, Greenberg J, Burke A, Alavi A (1987): Metabolic and clinical correlates of acute ischemic infarction. Neurology 37:1103–1110Google Scholar
  33. Mayeux R, Kandel ER (1985): Natural language, disorders of language and other localizable disorders of cognitive function. In: Principles of Neural Science, 2nd Edition, Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, eds. New York: Elsevier North HollandGoogle Scholar
  34. Menn L, Obier LK (1990): Agrammatic Aphasia: A Cross Language Narrative Sourcebook. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
  35. Mohr JP (1976): Broca’s area and Broca’s aphasia. In: Studies in Neurolinguistics, Vol. 1, Whitaker H, Whitaker HA, eds. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  36. Petersen SE, Robinson DL, Morris JD (1987): Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention. Neuropsychologia 25:97–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Poizner H, Klima ES, Bellugi U (1987): What the Hands Reveal about the Brain. Boston: Bradford BooksGoogle Scholar
  38. Posner MI, and Petersen SE (1990): The attentional system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuro science s 13:25–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Posner MI, Petersen SE, Fox PT, Raichle ME (1988): Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science 240:1627–1631CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Roeltgen D (1985): Agraphia. In: Clinical Neuropsychology, Heilman KM, Valen-Stein E, eds. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  41. Ross ED (1981): The aprosodias. Archives of Neurology 38:561–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ryalls JH (1986): A study of vowel production in aphasia. Brain and Language 29:48–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Saffran EM (1982): Neuropsychological approaches to the study of language. British Journal of Psychology 73:317–337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Saffran EM, Berndt RS, and Schwartz MF (1989): The quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: Procedures and data. Brain and Language 37:440–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sergent J (1983): Role of the input in visual hemispheric asymmetries. Psychological Bulletin 93:481–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sergent J (1987): A new look at the human split brain. Brain 110:1375–1392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sergent J (1988): Some theoretical and methodological issues in neuropsychological research. In: Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 1, Boiler F, Grafman J, eds. Amsterdam: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
  48. Sergent J (1989): Image generation and processing of generated images in the cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 15:170–178Google Scholar
  49. Sergent J (1990a): Furtive incursions into bicameral minds. Brain 113:537–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sergent J (1990b): The neuropsychology of visual image generation: data, method and theory. Brain and Cognition 13:98–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Springer SP, Deutsch G (1981): Left Brain, Right Brain. San Francisco: FreemanGoogle Scholar
  52. Tonkonogy V, Goodglass H (1981): Language function, foot of the third frontal gyrus, and rolandic operculum. Archives of Neurology 38:486–490CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Ungerleiter LC, Mishkin M (1982): Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of Visual Behavior, Ingle DT, Goodale M A, Mansfield RJW, eds. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  54. Wernicke C (1874): Der Aphasiche Symptomencomplex. Breslaer: Cohn and WeigartGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Birkhäuser Boston 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rita Sloan Berndt

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations