Advertisement

Bibliography

  • Steven E. Hampson

Keywords

Latent Inhibition Classical Conditioning Associative Learning Animal Learning Associative Memory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Bibliography

  1. Abraham, W. C., Goddard, G. V. (1983): Asymmetric relations between homosynaptic long-term potentiation and heterosynaptic long-term depression. Nature (London) 305, 717–719CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abraham, W. C., Goddard, G. V. (1984): Functions of afferent coactivity in long-term potentiation. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  3. Abraham, W. C., Goddard, G. V. (1985): Multiple traces of neural activity in the hippocampus. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  4. Abrams, T. W. (1985): Cellular studies of an associative mechanism for classical conditioning in Aplysia. In: Model neural networks and behavior. Selverston, A. I. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  5. Ackley, D. H., Hinton, G. E., Sejnowski, T. J. (1985): A learning algorithm for Boltzmann machines. Cog. Sci. 9, 147–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Adams, C., Dickinson, A. (1981): Actions and habits: Variations in associative representations during instrumental learning. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  7. Adrian, E. D (1946): The physical background of perception. Oxford, Eng: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Aggleton, J. P., Mishkin, M. (1986): The amygdala: sensory gateway to the emotions. In: Emotion: vol 3. Biological foundations. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  9. Agranoff, B. W. (1982): Learning, memory formation and protein synthesis. In: Changing concepts of the nervous system. Morrison, A. R., Strick, P. L. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  10. Ajzen, I., Fishbein, M. (1975): A Baysian analysis of attribution processes. Psych. Bull. 82, 262–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Albus, J. S. (1979): Mechanisms of planning and problem solving in the brain. Math. Biosci. 45, 247–293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Albus, J. S. (1981): Brains, behavior and robotics. Peterborough, NH: BYTE/McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  13. Alger, B. E., Teyler, T. J. (1976): Long-term and short-term plasticity in CA1, CA3 and dentate regions of the rat hippocampal slice. Brain Res. 110, 463–480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Alkon, D. L. (1986): Changes of membrane currents and calcium-dependent phosphorylation during associative learning. In: Neural mechanisms of conditioning. Alkon, D. L., Woody, C. D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Alkon, D. L., Quek, F., Vogel, T. P. (1989): Computer modeling of associative learning. In: Neural information processing systems I. Touretzky, D. S (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  16. Allan, L. G. (1980): A note on measurement of contingency between two binary variables in judgement tasks. Bull. Psychonomic Society 15, 147–149Google Scholar
  17. Alloy, L. B., Seligman, M. E. P. (1979): On the cognitive component of learned helplessness and depression. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 13. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  18. Alloy, L. B., Tabachnik, N. (1984): Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. Psych. Rev. 91, 112–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Amari, S. (1972a): Characteristics of random nets of analog neuron-like elements. IEEE Trans. Sys. Man Cyber. smc-2 643–657Google Scholar
  20. Amari, S. (1972b): Learning patterns and pattern sequences by self-organizing nets of threshold elements. IEEE Trans. Computers C21, 1197–1206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Amari, S. (1977): Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biol. Cyber. 27, 77–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Amari, S. (1980): Topographic organization of nerve fields. Bull. Math. Biol. 42, 339–364Google Scholar
  23. Amari, S., Arbib, M. A. (1977): Competition and cooperation in neural nets. In: Systems Neuroscience. Metzler, J. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  24. Amari, S., Arbib, M. A. (eds.) (1982): Competition and cooperation in neural nets. New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  25. Amiro, T. W., Bitterman, M. E. (1980): Second-order appetitive conditioning in goldfish. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 6, 41–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Amsel, A. (1958): The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psych. Bull 55, 102–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Amsel, A., Stanton, M. (1980): Ontogeny and phylogeny of paradoxical reward effects. In: Advances in the study of behavior. vol. II. IIRosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C, Musnel, M. C. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  28. Amsel, A., Heiden, U. (1980): Analysis of neural networks, lecture notes in biomathematics 35. New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  29. Anderson, C. W. (1982): Feature generation and selection by a layered network of reinforcement learning elements: Some initial experiments. Comp. and Info. Sci. Dept. U. of Mass. Amherst, Mass. tech report 82–112Google Scholar
  30. Anderson, D. C., O’Farrell, T., Formica, R., Caponegri, V. (1969a): Preconditioning CS exposure: variation in the place of conditioning and presentation. Psychon. Sci. 15, 54–55Google Scholar
  31. Anderson, D. C, Wolf, D., Sullivan, P. (1969b): Preconditioning exposures to the CS: Variations in place of testing. Psychon. Sci. 14, 233–235Google Scholar
  32. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.) (1988): Neural information processing systems. New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  33. Anderson, J. A., Hinton, G. E. (1981): Models of information processing in the brain. In: Parallel models of associative memory. Hinton, G. E., Anderson, J. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  34. Anderson, J. R. (1976): Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  35. Anderson, J. R. (1982): Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psych. Rev. 89, 369–406Google Scholar
  36. Anderson, J. R. (1983): The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  37. Anderson, J. R. (1984): Spreading activation. In: Tutorials in learning and memory. Anderson, J. R., Kosslyn, S. M. (eds.). New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  38. Anderson, J. R., Bower, G. H. (1973): Human associative memory. New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  39. Anderson, P., Sunberg, S. H., Sveen, O., Swann, J. W., Wigstrom, H. (1980): Possible mechanisms for long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in hippocampal slices from guinea-pigs. Jour. Physiol. 302, 463–482Google Scholar
  40. Arbib, M. A. (1972): The metaphorical brain. New York: InterscienceGoogle Scholar
  41. Arbib, M. A. (1981): Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In: Handbook of physiology. sec. 1. The nervous system. vol. 2. Motor control. Brooks, V. B. (ed.). Bethesda, MD: American Physiol. SocietyGoogle Scholar
  42. Arbib, M. A., Lieblich, I. (1977): Motivational learning of spatial behavior. In: Systems neuroscience. Metzler, J. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  43. Artola, A., Singer, W. (1987): Long-term potentiation and NMDA receptors in rat visual cortex. Nature 330, 649–652CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ashby, F. G., Perrin, N. A. (1988): Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psych. Rev. 95, 124–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ashby, W. R. (1960): Design for a brain. London: Chapman and HallGoogle Scholar
  46. Atkinson, R. C, Estes, W. K. (1963): Stimulus sampling theory. In: Handbook of mathematical psychology vol. 2. Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., Galanter, E. (eds.). New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  47. Bachevalier, J., Mishkin, M. (1984): An early and late developing system for learning and retention in infant monkeys. Behav. Neurosci. 96, 770–776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Baker, A. G. (1976): Learned irrelevance and learned helplessness: Rats learn that stimuli reinforcers, and responses are uncorrelated. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 2, 130–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Baker, A. G., Baker, P. A. (1985): Does inhibition differ from excitation: Proactive interference, contextual conditioning, and extinction. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  50. Baker, A. G., Mackintosh, N. J. (1977): Excitatory and inhibitory conditioning following uncorrelated presentations of the CS and US. Animal Learning and Behavior 5, 315–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Baker, A. G., Mackintosh, N. J. (1979): Preexposure to the CS alone, US alone, or CS and US uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context or learned irrelevance. Learning and Motivation 10, 278–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Baker, A. G., Mercier, P. (1982a): Prior experience with the conditioning events: Evidence for a rich cognitive representation. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior (vol. III). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  53. Baker, A. G., Mercier, P. (1982b): Extinction of the context and latent inhibition. Learning and Motivation 13, 391–416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Balsam, P. D. (1985): The functions of context in learning and performance. In: Context and learning. Balsam, P. D., Tomie, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  55. Barlow, H. B. (1972): Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology. Perception 1, 371–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L. (1985): Spatial information: How and where is it stored? In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  57. Barrionuevo, G., Brown, T. H. (1983): Associative long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 80, 7347–7351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Barsalou, L. W. (1985): Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 11, 625–654Google Scholar
  59. Barsalou, L. W., Bower, G. H. (1984): Discrimination nets as psychological models. Cog. Sci. 8, 1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Barto, A. G. (1985): Learning by statistical cooperation of self-interested neuron-like computing elements. Human Neurobiol. 4, 229–256Google Scholar
  61. Barto, A. G., Sutton, R. S. (1982): Simulation of anticipatory responses in classical conditioning by a neuron-like adaptive element. Behav. Brain Res. 4, 221–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Barto, A. G., Sutton, R. S., Anderson, C. W. (1982): Neuron-like adaptive elements that can solve difficult learning control problems. Comp. and Info. Sci. Dept. U. of Mass. Amherst, Mass. tech report 82–120Google Scholar
  63. Barto, A. G., Sutton, R. S., Brouwer, P. S. (1981): Associative search network: A reinforcement learning associative memory. Biol. Cyber. 40, 201–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Bassler, U. (1986): On the definition of central pattern generator and its sensory control. Biol. Cyber. 54, 65–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Baudry, M., Arst, D. S., Lynch, G. (1981): Increased glutamate receptor binding in aged rats. Brain Research 223, 195–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Baudry, M., Lynch, G. (1984): Glutamate receptor regulation and the substrates of memory. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  67. Beach, L. R. (1964a): Cue probabilism and inference behavior. Psych. Monographs 78(5), 1–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Beach, L. R. (1964b): Recognition, assimilation, and identification of objects. Psych. Monographs 78(6), 21–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Bellingham, W. P., Gillette-Bellingham, K., Kehoe, E. J. (1985): Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rat and rabbit. Animal Learning and Behavior 13, 152–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Belnap, N. D. (1977): A useful four-valued logic. In: Modern uses of multiple-valued logic. Dunn, J. M., Epstein, G. (eds.). Boston, MA: Reidel PublishingGoogle Scholar
  71. Ben-Ari, Y. (ed.) (1981): The amygdaloid complex. New York: Elsevier-North HollandGoogle Scholar
  72. Bentley, D., Konishi, M. (1978): Neural control of behavior. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 35–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Berman, R. F. (1986): Studies of memory processes using electrical brain stimulation. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  74. Berman, R. F., Kesner, R. P. (1976): Posttrial hippocampal, amygdaloid, and lateral hypothalamic electrical stimulation: Effect on short-and long-term memory of an appetitive experience. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 90, 260–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Bindman, L., Lippold, O. (1981): The neurophysiology of the cerebral cortex. Austin, Texas: Texas Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  76. Bindman, L. J., Lippold, O. C. J., Milne, A. R. (1979): Prolonged changes in excitability of pyramidal tract neurons in the cat: A post-synaptic mechanism. Jour. Physiol. (London) 286, 457–477Google Scholar
  77. Bindman, L. J., Prince, C. A. (1986): Persistent changes in excitability and input resistance of cortical neurons in the rat. In: Neural mechanisms of conditioning. Alkon, D. L., Woody, C. D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  78. Bindra, D. (1974): A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psych. Rev. 81, 199–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Bindra, D. (1976): A theory of intelligent behavior. New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  80. Bindra, D. (1978): How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 41–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Bitterman, M. E. (1975): The comparative analysis of learning. Science 188, 699–709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Bitterman, M. E. (1979): Attention. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  83. Black, J. E., Greenough, W. T. (1986): Developmental approaches to the memory process. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  84. Bliss, T. V. P., Dolphin, A. C. (1984): Where is the locus of long-term potentiation? In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  85. Blodgett, H. C. (1929): The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psych. 4, 113–134Google Scholar
  86. Blum, A., Rivest, R. L. (1989): Training a 3-node neural network is NP-complete. In: Neural information processing systems I. Touretzky, D. S. (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  87. Bolles, R. C. (1972): Reinforcement, expectancy and learning. Psych. Rev. 79, 394–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Boneau, C. A. (1974): Paradigm regained? Cognitive behaviorism restated. Amer. Psych. 29, 297–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Bourne Jr. L. E., Restle, F. (1959): Mathematical theory of concept identification. Psych. Rev. 66, 278–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Bourne Jr. L. E. (1970): Knowing and using concepts. Psych. Rev. 77, 546–556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Bourne Jr. L. E., Dominowski, R. L., Loftus, E. F. (1979): Cognitive Processes. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice HallGoogle Scholar
  92. Bouton, M. E., Swartzentruber, D. (1986): Analyses of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 12, 333–350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Bower, G. H. (1959): Choice point behavior. In: Studies in mathematical learning theory. Bush, R. R., Estes, W. K. (eds.). Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  94. Bower, G. H., Hilgard, E. R., (1981): Theories of learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice HallGoogle Scholar
  95. Breland, K., Breland, M. (1961): The misbehavior of organisms. Amer. Psych. 61, 681–694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Brindley, C. S. (1967): Classification of modifiable synapses and their use in models for conditioning. Proc. Royal Soc. London B. 168, 361–376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Brogden, W. J. (1939): Sensory pre-conditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych. 25, 323–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Brons, J. F., Woody, C. D. (1980): Long-term changes in excitability of cortical neurons after Pavlovian conditioning and extinction. Jour. Neurophysiol. 44, 605–615Google Scholar
  99. Brons, J. F., Woody, C. D., Allon, N. (1982): Changes in the excitability of weak intensity electrical stimulation of units of the pericruciate cortex in cats. Jour. Neurophysiol. 47, 377–388Google Scholar
  100. Brown, M. F. (1987): Dissociation of stimulus compounds by pigeons. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 13, 80–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Brown, R., Kulik, J. (1977): Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5, 73–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Brown, T. H., Ganong, A. H., Kairiss, E. W., Keenan, C. L., Kelso, S. R. (1989): Long-term potentiation in two synaptic systems of the hippocampal brain slice. In: Neural models of plasticity. Byrne, J. H., Berry, W. O. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  103. Brunswick, E. (1939): Probability as a determiner of rat behavior. Jour. Exp. Psych. 25, 175–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Bulgarella, R. G., Archer, E. J. (1962): Concept identification of auditory stimuli as a function of amount of relevant and irrelevant information. Jour. Exp. Psych. 63, 254–257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Bures, J., Buresova, O. (1970): Plasticity in single neurons and neural populations. In: Short-term changes in neural activity and behavior. Horn, G., Hinde, R. A. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  106. Busemeyer, J. R., Dewey, G. I., Medin, D. L. (1984): Evaluation of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 10, 638–648CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Busemeyer, J. R., Myung, I. J. (1988): A new method for investigating prototype learning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 14, 3–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Byrne, J. H., Gingrich, K. J. (1989): Mathematical model of cellular and molecular processes contributing to associative and non associative learning in Aplysia. In: Neural models of plasticity. Byrne, J. H., Berry, W. O. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  109. Carew, T. J. (1987): Cellular and molecular advances in the study of learning in Aplysia. In: The neural and molecular basis of learning. Changeaux, J. P., Konishi, M. (eds.). New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  110. Carew, T. J., Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R. (1983): Differential classical conditioning of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. Science 219, 397–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Carley, L. R. (1988): Presynaptic neural information processing. In: Neural information processing systems. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  112. Carlson, N. R. (1980): Physiology of behavior. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.Google Scholar
  113. Carlson, N. R. (1986): Physiology of behavior. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.Google Scholar
  114. Chan-Palay, V., Palay, S. L. (1984): Coexistence of neuroactive substances in neurons. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  115. Channell, S., Hall, G. (1983): Contextual effects in latent inhibition with an appetitive conditioning procedure. Animal Learning and Behavior 11, 67–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Chapouthier, G. (1983): Protein synthesis and memory. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  117. Chen, H. H., Lee, Y. C, Sun, G. Z., Lee, H. Y., Maxwell, T., Giles, C. L. (1986): High order correlation model for associative memory. In: Neural networks for computing. Denker, J. S. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  118. Cheng, K., Gallistel, C. R. (1984): Testing the geometric power of an animal’s spatial representation. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  119. Clark, J. W. (1988): Probabilistic neural networks. In: Evolution, learning and cognition. Lee, Y. C. (ed.). Teaneck, NJ: World ScientificGoogle Scholar
  120. Cohen, A. H., Rossignol, S., Grillner, S. (eds.) (1986): Neural control of rhythmic movements. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  121. Cohen, B., Murphy, G. L. (1984): Models of concepts. Cog. Sci. 8, 27–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Cohen, N. J. (1984): Preserved learning capacity in amnesia: Evidence for multiple memory systems. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  123. Cohen, N. J., McCloskey, M., Wible, C. G. (1988): There is still no case for a flashbulb-memory mechanism: Reply to Schmidt and Bohannon (1988) Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 336–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Cohen, N. J., Squire, L. R. (1980): Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210, 207–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Cohen, P. R., Feigenbaum, E. A. (1982): The handbook of artificial intelligence, vol. 3. Stanford, CA: Heuristech PressGoogle Scholar
  126. Collett, T. S. (1983): Sensory guidance of motor behavior. In: Animal behavior: 1 Causes and effects. Halliday, T. R., Slater, P. J. B. (eds.). New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  127. Collins, A. M., Loftus, E. F. (1975): A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psych. Rev. 82, 407–429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Collins, A. M., Quillian, M. R. (1969): Retrieval time from semantic memory. Jour. of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8, 240–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Collins, A., Quillian, M. R. (1972): Experiments on semantic memory and language comprehension. In: Cognition in learning and memory. Gregg, L. W. (ed.). New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  130. Collins, L., Pearce, J. M. (1985): Predictive accuracy and the effects of partial reinforcement on serial autoshaping. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 548–564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Colwill, R. M., Rescorla, R. A. (1985a): Post-conditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 120–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Colwill, R. M., Rescorla, R. A. (1985b): Instrumental responding remains sensitive to reinforcer devaluation after extensive training. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 520–536CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Colwill, R. M., Rescorla, R. A. (1985c): Extensive training, partial reinforcement, and temporal gaps do not affect S-S learning in second-order autoshaping. Animal Learning and Behavior 13, 165–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Colwill, R. M., Rescorla, R. A. (1986): Associative structures in instrumental learning. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 20. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  135. Cotman, C. W., Monaghan, D. T., Ganong, A. H. (1988): Excitatory amino acid neurotransmission: NMDA receptors and Hebb-type synaptic plasticity. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 61–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Cowan, W. M. (1979): The development of the brain. Sei. Amer. 241, 112–133Google Scholar
  137. Crespi, L. P. (1942): Quantitative variation in incentive and performance in the white rat. Amer. Jour. Psych. 55, 467–517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Crick, F. (1984): Function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 81, 4586–4590CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Crick, F. H. C, Asanuma, C. (1986): Certain aspects of the anatomy and physiology of the cerebral cortex. In: Parallel distributed processing vol. 2. McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  140. Crutcher, K. A. (1986): Anatomical correlates of neuronal plasticity. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  141. D’Amato, M. R., Safarjan, W. R., Salmon, D. (1981): Long-delay conditioning and instrumental learning: Some new findings. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  142. D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P. (1984): Cognitive process in cebus monkeys. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  143. Damianopoulos, E. N. (1982): Necessary and sufficient factors in classical conditioning. Pavlovian Jour. Biol. Sci. 17, 215–229Google Scholar
  144. Davidson, T. L., Rescorla, R. A. (1986): Transfer of facilitation in the rat. Animal Learning and Behavior. 14, 380–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Davis, M., Hitchcock, J. M., Rosen, J. B. (1987): Anxiety and the amygdala: pharmacological and anatomical analysis of the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 21. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  146. Davison, M., McCarthy, D. (1987): The matching law. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  147. Daw, N. W., Brunken, W. J., Parkinson, D. (1989): The function of synaptic transmitters in the retina. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 205–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Dawson, R. G., McGaugh, J. L. (1969): Electroconvulsive shock effect on a reactivated memory trace: further examination. Science 166, 525–527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Denker, J. S. (1986): Neural networks for computing. New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  150. Denny, M. R. (1971): Relaxation theory and experiments. In: Aversive conditioning and learning. Brush, F. R. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  151. Depue, R. A., Iacono, W. G. (1989): Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Ann. Rev. Psych. 40, 457–492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Desimone, R., Moran, J., Spitzer, H. (1989): Neural mechanisms of attention in extrastriate cortex of monkeys. In: Dynamic interactions in neural networks: models and data. Arbib, M. A., Amari, S. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  153. Dethier, V. G. (1976): The hungry fly. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  154. Deutsch, D. (1972): Effect of repetition of standard and comparison tones on recognition memory for pitch. Jour. Exp. Psych. 93, 156–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Deutsch, D. (1984): Memory for nonverbal auditory information: A link between behavioral and physiological studies. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  156. Deutsch, J. A. (1960): The structural basis of behavior. Chicago, IL: Univ. of Chicago. PressGoogle Scholar
  157. Deutsch, J. A. (1963): Learning and electrical self-stimulation of the brain. Jour. Theor. Biol. 4, 193–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Deutsch, J. A. (1983): Dietary control and the stomach. Prog. in Neurobiol. 20: 313–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Deutsch, J. A., Deutsch, D. (1966): Physiological psychology. Homewood, IL: Dorsey PressGoogle Scholar
  160. Deutsch, J. A., Deutsch, D. (1973): Physiological psychology. second edition. Homewood, IL: Dorsey PressGoogle Scholar
  161. Deutsch, J. A., Howarth, C. I. (1963): Some tests of a theory of intercranial self-stimulation. Psych. Rev. 70, 444–460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. DeVietti, T. L., Bauste, R. L., Nutt, G., Barrett, O. V., Daly, K., Petree, A. D. (1987): Latent inhibition: A trace conditioning phenomenon? Learning and Motivation 18, 185–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. DeVietti, T. L., Conger, G. L., Kirkpatrick, B. R. (1977): Comparison of the enhancement gradients of retention obtained with stimulation of the mesencephalic reticular formation after training or memory reactivation. Physiology and Behavior 19, 549–554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  164. Dexter, W. R., Merrill, H. K. (1969): Role of contextual discrimination in fear conditioning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 69, 677–681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  165. Diamond, D. M., Weinberger, N. M. (1984): Physiological plasticity of single neurons in auditory cortex of the cat during acquisition of the pupillary conditioned response: II. Secondary field (AII). Behav. Neurosci. 98, 182–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Diamond, I. T. (1979): The subdivisions of neocortex: A proposal to revise the traditional view of sensory, motor and association areas. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology. vol. 8. Sprague, J. M., Epstein, A. N. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  167. Dickinson, A. (1980): Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  168. Dickinson, A. (1985): Actions and habits: The development of behavioral autonomy. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 308, 67–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Dickinson, A., Mackintosh, N. J. (1978): Classical conditioning in animals. Ann. Rev. Psych. 29, 587–612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Dickinson, A., Mackintosh, N. J. (1979): Reinforcer specificity in the enhancement of conditioning by postrial surprise. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 5, 162–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. (1985): Animal conditioning and human causality judgements. In: Perspectives on learning and memory. Nilsson, L., Archer, T. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  172. Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. R., Evenden, J. L. (1984): Judgement of act-outcome contingency: the role of selective attribution. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 36A, 29–50Google Scholar
  173. Didday, R. L., Arbib, M. A. (1975): Eye movements and visual perception: A “two visual system” model. Int. Jour. of Man-Machine Studies 7, 547–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. Disterhoft, J. F., Olds, J. (1972): Differential development of conditioned unit changes in thalamus and cortex of rat. Jour. Neurophysiol. 35, 665–679Google Scholar
  175. Disterhoft, J. F., Quinn, K. J., Weiss, C. (1987): Analyses of the auditory input and motor output pathways in rabbit nictitating membrane conditioning. In: Classical conditioning (3rd ed.). Gormezano, I., Prokasy, W. F., Thompson, R. F. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  176. Disterhoft, J. F., Stuart, D. K. (1976): Trial sequence of changed unit activity in auditory system of alert rat during conditioned response acquisition and extinction. Jour. Neurophysiol. 39, 266–281Google Scholar
  177. Domjan, M. (1980): Ingestional aversion learning: Unique and general processes. In: Advances in the study of behavior. Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C, Busnel, M. C. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  178. Domjan, M. (1983): Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: Implications for general process theory. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 11. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  179. Domjan, M., Burkhard, B. (1982): The principles of learning and behavior. Monterey, CA.: Brooks/Cole Publishing CompanyGoogle Scholar
  180. Domjan, M., Burkhard, B. (1986): The principles of learning and behavior. Monterey, CA.: Brooks/Cole Publishing CompanyGoogle Scholar
  181. Domjan, M., Galef, B. G. Jr. (1983): Constraints on instrumental classical conditioning: Retrospect and prospect. Animal Learning and Behavior 11, 151–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  182. Douglas, R. J. (1967): The hippocampus and behavior. Psych. Bull. 67, 416–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  183. Douglas, R. J. (1972): Pavlovian conditioning and the brain. In: Inhibition and learning. Boakes, R. A., Halliday, M. S. (eds.). London: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  184. Doyle, J. (1979): A truth maintenance system. Artif. Intel. 12, 231–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Duda, R. D., Hart, P. E. (1973): Pattern classification and scene analysis. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  186. Dunwiddie, T., Lynch, G. (1978): Long-term potentiation and depression of synaptic responses in the rat hippocampus: Localization and frequency dependency. Jour. Physiol. 276, 353–367Google Scholar
  187. Durlach, P. J., Rescorla, R. A. (1980): Potentiation rather than overshadowing in flavor-aversion learning: An analysis in terms of within-compound associations. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 6, 175–187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  188. Edelman, G. M., Gall, W. E., Cowan, W. M. (eds.). (1985): Molecular bases of neural development. New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  189. Erickson, R. P. (1974): Parallel “population” neural coding in feature extraction. In: The neurosciences. Schmitt, F. O., Worden, F. G. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  190. Erickson, R. P. (1982): The across-fiber pattern theory: An organizing principle for molar neural function. Contributions to Sensory Physiology 6, 79–110Google Scholar
  191. Esposito, R. V. (1984): Cognitive-affect integration: some recent trends from a neurobiological perspective. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H. Parker, E. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  192. Estes, W. K. (1960): A random-walk model for choice behavior. In: Mathematical models in the social sciences. Arrow, K. J., Karlin, S., Suppes, P. (eds.). Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  193. Estes, W. K. (1962): Theoretical treatment of differential reward in multiple-choice learning and two-person interactions. In: Mathematical methods in small group processes. Criswell, J., Solomon, H., Suppes, P. (eds.). Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  194. Estes, W. K. (1972): Reinforcement in human behavior. Amer. Sci. 60, 723–729Google Scholar
  195. Estes, W. K. (1976a): The cognitive side of probability learning. Psych. Rev. 83, 37–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  196. Estes, W. K. (1976b): Some functions of memory in probability learning and choice behavior. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 10. Bower G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  197. Evans, S. H. (1967): A brief statement of schema theory. Psychon. Sci. 8(2): 87–88Google Scholar
  198. Fahlman, S. E. (1979): Netl: A system for representing and using real-world knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  199. Farley, J. (1986): Cellular mechanisms of causal detection in a mollusk. In: Neural mechanisms of conditioning. Alkon, D. L., Woody, C. D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  200. Farley, J. (1988): Causal detection in a mollusc: Cellular mechanisms of predictive coding, associative learning, and memory. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior VIII. Commons, M. L., Church, R. M., Stellar, J. R., Wagner, A. R., (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  201. Feldman, J. A. (1981): A connectionist model of visual memory. In: Parallel models of associative memory. Hinton, G. E., Anderson, J. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  202. Feldman, J. A. (1982): Dynamic connections in neural networks. Biol. Cyber. 46, 27–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  203. Feldman, J. A., Ballard, D. H. (1982): Connectionist models and their properties. Cog. Sc. 6, 205–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  204. Fisher, D. L. (1982): Limited-channel models of automatic detection: Capacity and scanning in visual search. Psych. Rev. 89, 662–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  205. Flaherty, C. F. (1982): Incentive contrasts: A review of behavioral changes following shifts in reward. Animal Learning and Behavior 10, 409–440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  206. Fonberg, E. (1986): Amygdala, emotions, motivation, and depressive states. In: Emotion: Biological foundations. vol. 3. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  207. Foote, S. L., Bloom, F. E., Aston-Jones, G. (1983): The nucleus locus coeruleus: new evidence of anatomical and physiological specificity. Phys. Rev. 63, 844–914Google Scholar
  208. Frey, P. W., Sears, R. J. (1978): Model of conditioning incorporating the Rescorla-Wagner associative axiom, a dynamic attentional process, and a catastrophe rule. Psych. Rev. 85, 321–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  209. Fukushima, K. (1975): Cognitron: A self-organizing multilayered neural network. Biol Cyber. 20, 121–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  210. Fukushima, K. (1980): Neocognitron: A self-organizing neural network model for a mechanism of pattern recognition unaffected by shift in position. Biol. Cyber. 36, 193–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  211. Fukushima, K. (1984): A hierarchical neural network model for associative memory. Biol. Cyber. 50, 105–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  212. Fukushima, K., Miyake, S. (1978): A self-organizing neural network with a function of associative memory: Feedback-type cognitron. Biol. Cyber. 28, 201–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  213. Fukushima, K., Miyake, S. (1982): Neocognitron: A self-organizing neural network model for a mechanism of visual pattern recognition. In: Competition and cooperation in neural nets. Amari, S., Arbib, M. A. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  214. Fuster, J. M. (1980): The prefrontal cortex. New York: RavenGoogle Scholar
  215. Fuster, J. M. (1984): The cortical substrate of memory. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  216. Fuster, J. M., Jervey, J. P. (1982): Neuronal firing in the inferotemporal cortex of the monkey in a visual memory task. Jour. Neurosci. 2, 361–375Google Scholar
  217. Gabriel, M., Foster, K., Orona, E., Saltwick, S. E., Stanton, M. (1980): Neuronal activity of cingulate cortex, anteroventral thalamus, and hippocampal formation in discriminative conditioning: Encoding and extraction of the significance of conditional stimuli. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology. vol. 9. Sprague, J. M., Epstein, A. N. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  218. Gabriel, M., Orona, E., Foster, K. (1982): Mechanism and generality of stimulus significance coding in a mammalian model system. In: Advances in behavioral biology vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  219. Gallistel, C. R. (1964): Electrical self-stimulation and its theoretical implications. Psych. Bull. 61, 23–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  220. Gallistel, C. R. (1973): Self-stimulation: The neurophysiology of reward and motivation. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  221. Gallistel, C. R. (1980): The organization of action. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  222. Gallistel, C. R. (1983): Self-stimulation. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  223. Gallistel, C. R. (1989): Animal cognition: The representation of space, time and number. Ann. Rev. Psych. 40, 155–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  224. Gallistel, C. R., Shizgal, P., Yeomans, J. S. (1981): A portrait of the substrate for self-stimulation. Psych. Rev. 88, 228–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  225. Gallistel, C. R., Stellar, J. R., Bubis, E. (1974): Parametric analysis of brain stimulation reward in the rat. I. The transient process and and the memory-containing process. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 87, 848–860CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  226. Garcia, J., Koelling, R. A. (1966): Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychon. Sc. 4, 123–124Google Scholar
  227. Garcia, J., Rusiniak, K. W., Kiefer, S. W., Bermudez-Rattoni, F. (1982): The neural integration of feeding and drinking habits. In: Advances in behavioral biology. vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  228. Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P., Mackintosh, N. J., Goodall, G., Cotton, M. M., Feldon, J. (1984): Successful overshadowing and blocking in hippocampectomized rats. Behav. Brain Res. 12, 29–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  229. Gati, I., Tversky, A. (1982): Representations of qualitative and quantitative dimensions. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Perception and Performance 8, 325–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  230. Gelperin, A., Hopfield, J. J., Tank, D. W. (1985): The logic of Limax learning. In: Model neural networks and behavior. Selverston, A. I. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  231. Gescheider, G. A. (1988): Psychophysical scaling. Ann. Rev. Psych. 39, 169–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  232. Gibbon, J. (1981): The contingency problem in autoshaping. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S., Gibbon, J. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  233. Gibbon, J., Berryman, R., Thompson, R. L. (1974): Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. Jour. Exp. Analysis Behav. 21, 585–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  234. Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., Fairhurst, S., Kacelnik, A. (1988): Scalar expectancy theory and choice between delayed rewards. Psych. Rev. 95, 102–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  235. Glazer, H. I., Weiss, J. M. (1976): Long-term interference effect: An alternative to “learned helplessness”. Jour. Exper. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 2, 201–213Google Scholar
  236. Gluck, M. A., Bower, G. H. (1988a): From conditioning to category learning: An adaptive network model. Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 227–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  237. Gluck, M. A., Bower, G. H. (1988b): Evaluating an adaptive network model of human learning. Jour. Memory and Language 27, 166–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  238. Gluck, M., Parker, D. B., Reifsnider, E. S. (1989): Learning with temporal derivatives in pulse-coded neuronal systems. In: Neural information processing systems I. Touretzky, D. S. (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufman.Google Scholar
  239. Gluck, M. A., Thompson, R. F. (1987): Modeling the neural substrates of associative learning and memory: A computational approach. Psych. Rev. 94, 176–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  240. Gold, P. E. (1984a): Memory modulation: Neurobiological contexts. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  241. Gold, P. E. (1984b): Memory modulation: Role of peripheral catecholamines. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  242. Gold, P. E., McGaugh, J. L. (1975): A single-trace, two-process view of memory storage. In: Short-term memory. Deutsch, D., Deutsch, J. A. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  243. Gold, P. E., McGaugh, J. L. (1984): Endogenous processes in memory consolidation. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  244. Gold, P. E., Zornetzer, S. F. (1983): The mnemon and its juices: Neuromodulation of memory processes. Behav. and Neural Biol. 38, 151–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  245. Gordon, W. C. (1981): Mechanisms of cue-induced retention enhancement. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  246. Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E. J. (1981): Classical conditioning and the law of contiguity. In: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Harzern, P., Zeiler, M. D. (eds.). New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  247. Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E. J. (1982): Associative transfer in classical conditioning to serial compounds. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior (vol. III). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  248. Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E. J., Marshall, B. S. (1983): Twenty years of classical conditioning research with the rabbit. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology. vol. 10. Sprague, J. M., Epstein, A. N. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  249. Goss, A. E., Wischner, G. J. (1956): Vicarious trial and error and related behavior. Psych. Bull. 53, 35–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  250. Gould, J. L. (1984): Natural history of honey bee learning. In: The biology of learning. Marler, P., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  251. Graf, P., Schacter, D. L. (1985): Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 11, 501–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  252. Granger, R. H., Schlimmer, J. C. (1986): The computation of contingency in classical conditioning. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 20. Bower G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  253. Gray, J. A. (1982): The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the function of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford, Eng: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  254. Gray, J. A. (1984): The hippocampus as an interface between cognition and emotion. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  255. Greenough, W. T. (1984): Possible structural substrates of plastic neural phenomena. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  256. Greenough, W. T. (1985): The possible role of experience-dependent synaptogenesis, or synapses on demand in the memory process. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N.M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  257. Grillner, S. (1981): Control of locomotion in bipeds, tetrapods and fish. In: Handbook of physiology. sec. 1. The nervous system. vol. 2. Motor control. Brooks, V. B. (ed.). Bethesda, MD: American Physiol. SocietyGoogle Scholar
  258. Grillner, S., Wallen, P. (1985): Central pattern generators for locomotion, with special reference to vertebrates. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 233–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  259. Grossberg, S. (1983): The quantized geometry of visual space: The coherent computation of depth, form, and lightness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6, 625–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  260. Grossberg, S. (1988a): Nonlinear neural networks: Principles, mechanisms, and architectures. Neural Networks 1, 17–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  261. Grossberg, S. (1988b): Competitive learning: From interactive activation to adaptive resonance. In: Connectionist models and their implications. Waltz, D., Feldman, J. A. (eds.). Norwood, NJ: Ablex PublishingGoogle Scholar
  262. Grossman, S. P. (1979): Role of the hypothalamus in the regulation of food and water intake. Psych. Rev. 82, 200–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  263. Grzywacz, N. M., Amthor, F. R. (1989): A computationally robust anatomical model for retinal directional selectivity. In: Neural information processing systems I. Touretzky, D. S. (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  264. Guthrie, E. R. (1952): The psychology of learning. New York: HarperGoogle Scholar
  265. Guyon, I., Personnaz, L., Nadal, J. P., Dreyfus, G. (1988): High order neural networks for efficient associative memory design. In: Neural information processing systems. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  266. Halgren, C. R. (1974): Latent inhibition in rats: associative or nonassociative? Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 86, 74–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  267. Hall, G. (1980): Exposure learning in animals. Psych. Bull. 88, 535–550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  268. Hall, G., Channell, S. (1985a): Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 470–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  269. Hall, G., Channeil, S. (1985b): Latent inhibition and conditioning after preexposure to the training context. Learning and Motivation 16, 381–397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  270. Hall, G., Channell, S. (1986): Context specificity of latent inhibition in taste aversion learning. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 35B, 121–139Google Scholar
  271. Hall, G., Kaye, H., Pearce, J. M. (1985): Attention and conditioned inhibition. In: Information processing in animals. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  272. Hall, G., Minor, H. (1984): A search for context-stimulus associations in latent inhibition. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 36B, 145–169Google Scholar
  273. Hall, G., Pearce, J. M. (1979): Latent inhibition of a CS during CS-US parings. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 5, 31–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  274. Hall, G., Pearce, J. M. (1982a): Restoring the associability of a pre-exposed CS by a surprising event. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 34B, 127–140Google Scholar
  275. Hall, G., Pearce, J. M. (1982b): Changes in stimulus associability during acquisition: Implications for theories of acquisition. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior (vol. III). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  276. Halperin, R., Pfaff, D. W. (1982): Brain-stimulation reward and control of autonomic function: are they related? In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  277. Hammond, L. J. (1980): The effect of contingency upon the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behavior. Jour. Exp. Analysis Behav. 34, 297–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  278. Hammond, L. J. (1985): An empirical legacy of two-process theory: Two-term versus three-term relations. In: Affect, conditioning, and cognition. Brush, F. R., Overmier, J. B. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  279. Hammond, L. J., Paynter, W. E. Jr. (1983): Probabilistic contingency theories of animal conditioning: A critical analysis. Learning and Motivation 14, 527–550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  280. Hampson, S. E. (1983): A neural model of adaptive behavior. PhD dissertation, Information and Computer Science Dept., Univ. of Calif., Irvin. Tech. Rep. 213Google Scholar
  281. Hampson, S. E., Kibler, D. (1983): A Boolean complete neural model of adaptive behavior. Biol. Cyber. 49, 9–19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  282. Hampson, S. E., Volper, D. J. (1986): Linear function neurons: Structure and training. Biol. Cyber. 53, 203–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  283. Hampson, S. E., Volper, D. J. (1987): Disjunctive models of Boolean category learning. Biol. Cyber. 56, 121–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  284. Hampson, S. E., Volper, D. J. (in press): Representing and learning Boolean functions of multivalued features. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cyber.Google Scholar
  285. Hawkins, R. D. (1983): Cellular neurophysiological studies of learning. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  286. Hawkins, R. D. (1989): A simple circuit model for higher-order features of classical conditioning. In: Neural models of plasticity. Byrne, J. H., Berry, W. O. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  287. Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R. (1984).: Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning? Psych. Rev. 91, 375–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  288. Hayes-Roth, F. (1978): The role of partial and best matches in knowledge systems. In: Pattern directed inference systems. Waterman, D. A., Hayes-Roth, F. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  289. Hearst, E. (1978): Stimulus relationships and feature selection in learning and behavior. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Huse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  290. Hearst, E. (1984): Absence as information: Some implications for learning, performance, and representational processes. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  291. Hearst, E. (1987): Extinction reveals stimulus control: Latent learning of feature-negative discriminations in pigeons. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 13, 52–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  292. Hebb, D. O. (1949): The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  293. Hecht-Nielsen, R. (1988): Applications of counterpropagation networks. Neural Networks 1, 131–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  294. Herrnstein, R. J. (1961): Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Jour. Exp. Analysis Behav. 4, 267–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  295. Herrnstein, R. J. (1970): On the law of effect. Jour. Exp. Analysis Behav. 13, 243–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  296. Herrnstein, R. J. (1974): Formal properties of the matching law. Jour. Exp. Analysis Behav. 21, 159–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  297. Herrnstein, R. J. (1985): Riddles of natural categorization. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 308, 129–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  298. Hilgard, E. R., Bower, G. H. (1975): Theories of learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice HallGoogle Scholar
  299. Hinde, R. A., Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1973): Constraints on learning. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  300. Hinton, G. E., Anderson, J. A. (eds.) (1981): Parallel models of associative memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  301. Hinton, G. E., Sejnowski, T. J., Ackley, D. H. (1984): Boltzmann machines: Constraint satisfaction networks that learn. Carnegie-Mellon Univ. tech. report CMU-CS-84-119 Google Scholar
  302. Hintzman, D. L. (1978): The psychology of learning and memory. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  303. Hintzman, D. L. (1986): “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psych. Rev. 93, 411–428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  304. Hintzman, D. L. (1988): Judgements of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psych. Rev. 95, 528–551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  305. Hintzman, D. L., Ludlam, G. (1980): Differential forgetting of prototypes and old instances: simulation of an exemplar based classification model. Memory and Cognition 8, 378–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  306. Hokfelt, T., Fux, K., Pernow, B. (eds.) (1986): Progress in brain research v. 68. Coexistence of neuronal messengers: A new principle in chemical transmission. New York: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
  307. Holland, J. H. (1986): Escaping brittleness. In: Machine learning II. Michalski, R. S., Carbonell, J. G., Mitchell, T. M. (eds.). Los Altos, CA: Morgan Kaufman Publishers Inc.Google Scholar
  308. Holland, P. C. (1982): Interelement associations in serial compound conditioning. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior (vol. 3). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
  309. Holland, P. C. (1983): Occasion-setting in Pavlovian feature positive discriminations. In: Quantitative analysis of behavior. (vol. 4). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). New York: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  310. Holland, P. C. (1984): Differential effects of reinforcement of an inhibitory feature after serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination training. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 10: 461–475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  311. Holland, P. C. (1985a): The nature of conditioned inhibition in serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  312. Holland, P. C. (1985b): Element pretraining influences the content of appetitive serial compound conditioning in rats. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 367–387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  313. Holland, P. C. (1986a): Transfer after serial feature positive discrimination training. Learning and Motivation 17: 243–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  314. Holland, P. C. (1986b): Temporal determinants of occasion setting in feature positive discriminations. Animal Learning and Behavior 14, 111–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  315. Holland, P. C, Lamarre, J. (1984): Transfer of inhibition after serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination training. Learning and Motivation 15, 219–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  316. Holland, P. C, Rescorla, R. A. (1975a): The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first-and second-order appetitive conditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 4: 355–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  317. Holland, P. C, Rescorla, R. A. (1975b): Second-order conditioning with food unconditioned stimuli. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 4: 355–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  318. Holland, P. C, Ross, R. T. (1981): Within-compound associations in serial compound conditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 7, 228–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  319. Holland, P. C, Ross, R. T. (1983): Savings test for associations between neutral stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior 11, 83–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  320. Homa, D. (1978): Abstraction of ill-defined form. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Learning and Memory 4, 407–416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  321. Homa, D., Chambliss, D. (1975): The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Learning and Memory 1, 351–359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  322. Homa, D., Cross, J., Cornell, D., Goldman, D., Schwartz, S. (1973): Prototype abstraction and classification of new instances. Jour. Exp. Psych. 101, 116–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  323. Homa, D., Sterling, S., Trepel, L. (1981): Limitations of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Learning and Memory 7, 418–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  324. Homa, D., Vosburgh, R. (1976): Category breadth and the abstraction of prototypical information. fI Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Learning and Memory/fP 2, 322–330Google Scholar
  325. Honey, R. C, Schachtman, T. R., Hall, G. (1987): Partial reinforcement in serial autoshaping: The role of attentional and associative factors. Learning and Motivation 18, 288–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  326. Honig, W. K. (1978): Studies of working memory in the pigeon. In: Cognitive processes in animai behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  327. Honig, W. K. (1984): Contributions of animal memory to the interpretation of animal learning. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  328. Hopfield, J. J. (1982): Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 79, 2554–2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  329. Hopfield, J. J., Tank, D. W. (1985): “Neural” computation of decisions in optimization problems. Biol. Cyber. 52, 141–152Google Scholar
  330. Howarth, C. L, Deutsch, J. A. (1962): Drive decay: The cause of fast “extinction” of habits learned from brain stimulation. Science 137, 35–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  331. Hoy, R,. R. (1989): Startle, categorical response, and attention in acoustic behavior of insects. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 355–375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  332. Hubel, D. H. (1981): Columns and their function is the primate visual cortex. In: Theoretical approaches in neurobiology. Reichardt, W. E., Poggio, T. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  333. Hull, C. L. (1932): The goal gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psych. Rev. 39, 25–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  334. Hull, C. L. (1952): A behavior system. New Haven, CN: Yale Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  335. Hulse, S. H., Egeth, H., Deese, J. (1980): The psychology of learning (fifth edition). New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  336. Hunt, E. (1989): Cognitive science: Definition, status, and questions. Ann. Rev. Psych. 40, 603–629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  337. Hunt, E., Martin, J., Store, P. (1966): Experiments in induction. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  338. Introini-Collison, I. B., McGaugh, J. L. (1986): Epinephrine modulates long-term retention of an aversively-motivated discrimination task. Behav. and Neural Biology 45, 358–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  339. Irwin, F. W. (1971): Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. Philadelphia, PN: LippincottGoogle Scholar
  340. Isaacson, R. L. (1982): The limbic system. New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  341. Jacobs, R. A. (1988): Increased rates of convergence through learning rate adaptation. Neural Networks 1, 295–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  342. Jenkins, H. M. (1985): Conditioned inhibition of key pecking in the pigeon. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  343. Jenkins, H. M., Barnes, R. A., Barrera, F. J. (1981): Why autoshaping depends on trial spacing. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S., Gibbon, J. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  344. Jenkins, H. M., Lambos, W. A. (1983): Tests of two explanations of response elimination by noncontingent reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior 11, 302–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  345. Jenkins, H. M., Sainbury R. S. (1970): Discrimination learning with the distinctive feature on positive and negative trials. In: Attention: Contempory theory and analysis. Mostofsky, D. (ed.). New York: Appleton-Century CroftsGoogle Scholar
  346. John, E. R. (1976): A model of consciousness. In: Consciousness and self-regulation. Schwartz, G. E., Shapiro, D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  347. John, E. R. (1980): A neurophysiological model of purposive behavior. In: Neural mechanism of goal-directed behavior and learning. Thompson, R. F., Hicks, L. H., Shvyrkow, V. B. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  348. John, E. R., Schwartz, E. I. (1978): The neurophysiology of information processing and cognition. Ann. Rev. Psych. 29, 1–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  349. Johnston, T. D. (1981): Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 125–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  350. Johnston, W. A., Dark, V. J. (1986): Selective attention. Ann. Rev. Psych. 37, 43–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  351. Jones, E. G. (1981a): Anatomy of cerebral cortex: Columnar input-output organization. In: The organization of the cerebral cortex. Schmitt, F. U., Worden, F. G., Adelman, G., Dennis, S. G. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  352. Jones, E. G. (1981b): Functional subdivisions and synaptic organization of the mammalian thalamus. In: Neurophysiology IV. Inter. rev. of physiol. vol. 25. Porter, R (ed.). Baltimore, MD: Univ. Park PressGoogle Scholar
  353. Jones, E. G. (1985): The thalamus. New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  354. Judge, M. E., Quartermain, D. (1982): Characteristics of retrograde amnesia following reactivation of memory in mice. Physiology and Behavior 28, 585–590CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  355. Kaczmarek, L. K., Levitan, I. B. (eds.). (1986): Neuromodulation: The biochemical control of neuronal excitability. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  356. Kamin, L. J. (1969): Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In: Punishment and aversive behavior. Campbell, B. A., Church, R. M. (eds.). New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  357. Kandel, E. R. (1976): Cellular basis of behavior. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  358. Kandel, E. R. (1977): Neuronal plasticity and the modification of behavior. In: Handbook of physiology, the nervous system. Kandel, E. R. (ed.). Baltimore, MD: Waverly Press Inc.Google Scholar
  359. Kandel, E. R. (1979a): Behavioral biology of Aplysia. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  360. Kandel, E. R. (1979b): Small systems of neurons. Sci. Amer. 241, 66–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  361. Kandel, E. R. (1979c): Cellular insights into behavior and learning. The Harvey lectures 73, 19–92Google Scholar
  362. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. (eds.) (1981): Principles of neural science. New York: Elsevier North-HollandGoogle Scholar
  363. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. (1982): Molecular biology of learning: Modulation of transmitter release. Science 218, 433–443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  364. Kaplan, P. S., Hearst, E. (1985): Contextual control and excitatory versus inhibitory learning: Studies of extinction, reinstatement and interference. In: Context and learning. Balsam, P. D., Tomie, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  365. Kapp, B. S., Gallagher, M., Applegate, C. D., Frysinger, R. C. (1982): The amygdala central nucleus: contributions to conditioned cardiovascular responding during aversive Pavlovian conditioning in the rabbit. In: Advances in behavioral biology. vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  366. Kapp, B. S., Pascoe, J. P. (1986): Memory: Vertebrate model systems. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  367. Kapp, B. S., Pascoe, J. P., Bixler, M. A. (1984): The amygdala: a neuroanatomical system approach to its contribution to aversive conditioning. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  368. Kasamatsu, T. (1983): Neuronal plasticity maintained by the central norepinephrine system in the cat visual cortex. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology. vol. 10. Sprague, J. M., Epstein, A. N. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  369. Kaye, H., Pearce, J. M. (1984a): The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 10, 90–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  370. Kaye, H., Pearce, J. M. (1984b): The strength of the orienting response during blocking. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 36B, 131–144Google Scholar
  371. Kaye, H., Pearce, J. M. (1987): Hippocampal lesions attenuate latent inhibition and the decline of the orienting response in rats. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 39B, 107–125Google Scholar
  372. Kaye, H., Preston, G. C., Szabo, L., Druiff, H., Mackintosh, N. J. (1987): Context specificity of conditioning and latent inhibition: Evidence for a dissociation of latent inhibition and associative interference. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 39B, 127–145Google Scholar
  373. Keeler, J. D. (1988): Capacity for patterns and sequences in Kanerva’s SDM as compared to other associative memory models. In: Neural information processing systems. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  374. Kehoe, E. J. (1988): A layered network model of associative learning: Learning to learn and configuration. Psych. Rev. 95, 411–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  375. Kehoe, E. J., Gormezano, I. (1980): Configuration and combination laws in conditioning with compound stimuli. Psych. Bull: 87, 351–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  376. Kehoe, E. J., Graham, P. (1988): Summation and configuration: Stimulus compounding and negative patterning in the rabbit. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 14, 320–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  377. Kehoe, E. J., Marshall-Goodell, G., Gormezano, I. (1987): Differential conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response to serial compound stimuli. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 13, 17–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  378. Kennedy, D., Davis, W. S. (1977): Organization of invertebrate motor systems. In: The cellular biology of neurons vol. 1, The nervous system sec. 1, The handbook of physiology. Kandel, E. Brookhart, M., Mountcastle, V. B. (eds.). Washington D. C: American Physiological SocietyGoogle Scholar
  379. Kent, E. W. (1981): The brains of men and machines. Peterborough, NH: BYTE/McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  380. Kesner, R. P. (1982): Brain stimulation: Effects on memory. Behav. and Neural Biol. 36, 315–367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  381. Kesner, R. P. (1986): Neurobiological views of memory. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  382. Kesner, R. P., DiMattia, B. V. (1987): Neurobiology of an attribute model of memory. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology vol. 12 Epstein, A. N., Morrison, A. R. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  383. Kety, S. S. (1982): The evolution of concepts of memory. In: The neural basis of behavior. Beckman, A. L. (ed.). New York: SP Medical and Scientific BooksGoogle Scholar
  384. Kim, E. H.-J., Woody, C. D., Berthier, N. E. (1983): Rapid acquisition of conditioned eye blink responses in cats following pairing of an auditory CS with glabella tap US and hypothalamic stimulation. Jour. Neurophysiol. 49, 767–779Google Scholar
  385. Kinsbourne, M., Wood, F. (1975): Short-term memory processes and the amnesic syndrome. In: Short-term memory. Deutsch, D., Deutsch, J. A. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  386. Kinsbourne, M., Wood, F. (1982): Theoretical considerations regarding the episodic-semantic memory distinction. In: Human and amnesia. Cermak, L. S. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  387. Kishimoto, K., Amari, S. (1979): Existence and stability of local excitations in neural fields. Jour. Math. Biol 7, 303–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  388. Klein, S. B. (1987): Learning. New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  389. Knudsen, E. I., DuLac, S., Esterly, S. D. (1987): Computational maps in the brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 41–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  390. Kohonen, T. (1977): Associative memory. New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  391. Kohonen, T. (1980): Content-addressable memories. New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  392. Kohonen, T. (1982a): Self-organized formation of topologically correct feature maps. Biol Cyber. 43, 59–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  393. Kohonen, T. (1982b): Analysis of a simple self-organizing process. Biol. Cyber. 44, 135–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  394. Kohonen, T. (1984): Self-organization and associative memory. New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  395. Kohonen, T., Oja, E., Lehtio, P. (1981): Storage and processing of information in distributed associative memory systems. In: Parallel models of associative memory. Hinton, G. E., Anderson, J. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  396. Kolb, B., Whishaw, I. Q. (1985): Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. New York: FreemanGoogle Scholar
  397. Konorski, J. (1967): Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago, IL: Univ. of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
  398. Kosslyn, S. M. (1980): Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  399. Kosslyn, S. M. (1987): Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computational approach. Psych. Rev. 94, 148–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  400. Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., Reiser, B. J. (1978): Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning. Jour. of Exper. Psych.: Human Perception and Performance 4, 47–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  401. Krasne, F. B. (1978): Extrinsic control of intrinsic neural plasticity. Brain Res. 140, 197–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  402. Kristan, W. B. (1980): Generation of rhythmic motor patterns. In: Information processing in the nervous system. Pinsker, H. M, Willis, W. D. (eds.). New York: Raven PressGoogle Scholar
  403. Kubie, J. L., Ranck, J. B. Jr. (1982): Tonic and phasic firing of rat hippocampal complex-spike cells in three different situations: Context and place. In: Advances in behavioral biology. vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  404. Kubie, J. L., Ranck, J. B. Jr. (1983): Sensory-behavioral correlates in individual hippocampus neurons in three situations: Space and context. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  405. Kubie, J. L., Ranck, J. B. Jr. (1984): Hippocampal neuronal firing, context and learning. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  406. Kubota, K. (1982): Prefrontal neuron activities, reversal and error performance. In: Advances in behavioral biology. vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  407. Kuffler, S. W., Nicholls, J. G., Martin, A. R. (1984): From neuron to brain: A cellular approach to the function of the nervous system. Sunderland, MA: Sinaver AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  408. Kupfermann, I. (1981): Hypothalamus and limbic system II: Motivation. In: Principles of neural science. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H. (eds.). New York: Elsevier/North-HollandGoogle Scholar
  409. LaBerge, D., Brown, V. (1989): Theory of attentional operations in shape identification. Psych. Rev. 96, 101–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  410. Lamarre, J., Holland, P. C. (1985): Acquisition of feature negative discriminations in rats conditioned suppression. Bull. Psychonomic Soc. 23, 71–74Google Scholar
  411. Lamarre, J., Holland, P. C. (1987): Transfer of inhibition after serial feature negative discrimination training. Learning and Motivation 18, 319–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  412. Lara, R., Arbib, M. A. (1985): A model of the neural mechanisms responsible for pattern recognition and stimulus specific habituation in toads. Biol. Cyber. 51, 223–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  413. Larkin, J. H., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., Simon, H. A. (1980): Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science 208, 1335–1342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  414. Lavin, M. J. (1976): The establishment of flavor-flavor associations using a sensory preconditioning training procedure. Learning and Motivation 7, 173–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  415. LeDoux, J. E. (1986): The neurobiology of emotion. In: Mind and brain. Le Doux, J. E., Hirst, W. (ed.). New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  416. Leong, H. M. (1985): Optimization with artificial neural network systems: A mapping principle and a comparison to gradient based methods. In: Neural information processing systems. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.). New York: American Institute of Physics (1988)Google Scholar
  417. Levine, D. N., Warach, J., Farah, M. J.: Two visual systems in mental imagery: Dissociation of “what” and “where” in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions. Neurology 35, 1010–1018Google Scholar
  418. Levy, W. B. (1985): Associative changes at the synapse: LTP in the hippocampus. In: Synaptic modification, neuron selectivity, and nervous system organization. Levy, W. B., Anderson, J. A., Lehmkuhle, S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  419. Lewis, D. J., Bregman, I. J., Mahan, J. J. (1972): Cue-dependent amnesia in rats. Jour. Comp. Physiol Psych. 81, 243–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  420. Lewis, D. J. (1979): Psychobiology of active and inactive memory. Psych. Bull. 86, 1054–1083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  421. Liang, K. C., Juler, R., McGaugh, J. L. (1986): Modulating effects of postraining epinephrine on memory: Involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system. Brain Res. 368, 125–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  422. Lieblich, I., Arbib, M. A. (1982): Multiple representations of space underlying behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5, 627–659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  423. Lindsay, P. H., Norman, D. A. (1977): Human information processing. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  424. Livingston, R. B. (1967a): Brain circuitry relating to complex behavior. In: The neurosciences. Quarton G. C, Melnechuck, T., Schmitt, F. O. (eds.). pg. 499–514. New York: Rockefeller Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  425. Livingston, R. B. (1967b): Reinforcement. In: The neurosciences. Quarton G. C., Melnechuck, T., Schmitt, F. O. (eds.). pg. 568–576. New York: Rockefeller Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  426. Loftus, E. F. (1982): Remembering recent experiences. In: Human memory and amnesia. Cermak, L. S. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  427. Loftus, E. F., Loftus, G. R. (1980): On the permanence of stored information in the human brain. Amer. Psych. 35, 409–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  428. Logue, A. W. (1979): Taste aversion and the generality of the laws of learning. Psych. Bull 86, 272–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  429. Lolordo, V. M. (1979a): Selective associations. In: Mechanisms of learning and motivation. Dickinson, A., Boakes, R. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  430. Lolordo, V. M. (1979b): Classical conditioning: The Pavlovian perspective. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  431. Lolordo, V. M. (1979c): Classical conditioning: Contingency and contiguity. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  432. Lolordo, V. M. (1979d): Classical conditioning: Compound CSs and the Rescorla-Wagner model. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  433. Lolordo, V. M. (1979e): Constraints on learning. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  434. Lolordo, V. M., Jacobs, W. J. (1983): Constraints on aversive conditioning in the rat: Some theoretical considerations. In: Advances in the analysis of behavior. Harzern, P., Zeiler, M. (eds.). London: WileyGoogle Scholar
  435. Lovibond, P. F., Preston, G. C, Mackintosh, N. J. (1984): Context specificity of conditioning, extinction, and latent inhibition. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 10, 360–375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  436. Lubow, R. E. (1973): Latent inhibition. Psych. Bull. 79, 398–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  437. Lubow, R. E., Moore, A. J. (1959).: Latent inhibition: the effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. Jour. Comp. Physiol Psych. 52, 416–419Google Scholar
  438. Lubow, R. E., Rifkin, B., Alek, M. (1976a): The context effect: The relationship between stimulus preexposure and environmental preexposure determines subsequent learning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 2, 38–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  439. Lubow, R. E., Schnur, P., Rifkin, B. (1976b): Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 2, 163–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  440. Lubow, R. E., Weiner, L, Schnur, P. (1981): Conditioned attention theory. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 15. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  441. Lund, R. D. (1978): Development and plasticity of the brain. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  442. Luria, A. R. (1980): Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic BooksCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  443. Lynch, G., Baudry, M. (1984): The biochemistry of memory: A new and specific hypothesis. Science 224, 1057–1063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  444. Lynch, G. S., Dunwiddie, T., Gribkoff, V. (1977): Heterosynaptic depression: A postsynaptic correlate of long-term potentiation. Nature 266, 737–739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  445. Lynn, R. (1966): Attention, arousal and the orientation reaction. New York: Pergamon PressGoogle Scholar
  446. MacFarlane, D. A. (1930): The role of kinesthesis in maze learning. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psych. 4, 277–305Google Scholar
  447. MacGregor, R. L. (1987): Neural and brain modeling. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  448. MacGregor, R. L., Lewis, E. R. (1977): Neural modeling. New York: Plenum PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  449. MacKay, D. M., McCulloch, W. S. (1952): The limiting information capacity of a neuronal link. Bull Math. Biophys. 14, 127–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  450. Mackintosh, N. J. (1973): Stimulus selection: learning to ignore stimuli that predict no change in reinforcement. In: Constraints on learning. Hinde, R. A., Stevenson-Hinde, J. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  451. Mackintosh, N. J. (1975): A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcements. Psych. Rev. 82, 276–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  452. Mackintosh, N. J. (1978): Cognitive or associative theories of conditioning: Implications of an analysis of blocking. In: Cognitive process in animal behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  453. Mackintosh, N. J. (1983): Conditioning and associative learning. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  454. Mackintosh, N. J. (1985a): Contextual specificity or state dependency of human and animal learning. In: Perspectives on learning and memory. Nilsson, L., Archer, T. (eds.) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  455. Mackintosh, N. J. (1985b): Varieties of conditioning. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  456. MacLean, P. D. (1986): Ictal symptoms relating to the nature of affects and their cerebral substrates. In: Emotion: vol. 3. Biological foundations. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  457. Mactutus, C. F., Riccio, D. C., Ferek, J. M. (1979): Retrograde amnesia for old (reactivated) memories: Some anomalous characteristics. Science 204, 1319–1320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  458. Mahut, H. (1985): Dissociation of two behavioral functions in the monkey after early hippocampal ablations. In: Brain plasticity, learning and memory. Will, B. E., Schmitt, P., Dairymple-Alford, J. C. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  459. Maier, S. F., Jackson, R. L. (1979): Learned helplessness: All of us were right (and wrong): Inescapable shock has multiple effects. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 13. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  460. Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E. P. (1976): Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 105, 3–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  461. Manning, A. (1976): Animal learning: Ethological approaches. In: Neural mechanisms of learning and memory. Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  462. Marr, D. (1982): Vision. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  463. Martinez, J. L. Jr. (1986): Memory: drugs and hormones. In: Learning and memory. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Kesner, R. P. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  464. Martinez, J. L. Jr., Jensen, R. A., McGaugh, J. L. (1983): Facilitation of memory consolidation. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  465. Matsuoka, K. (1985): Sustained oscillations generated by mutually inhibitory neurons with adaption. Biol. Cyber. 52, 367–376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  466. Matsuoka, K. (1987): Mechanisms of frequency and pattern control in the neural rhythm generators. Biol. Cyber. 56, 345–353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  467. Matthies, H. (1989): Neurobiological aspects of learning and memory. Ann. Rev. Psych. 40, 381–404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  468. Maunsell, J. H. R., Newsome, W. T. (1987): Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Ann. Rev. Neuros ci. 10, 363–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  469. Mazur, J. E. (1986): Learning and behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.Google Scholar
  470. McCauley, C, Stitt, C. L. (1978): An individual and quantitative measure of stereotypes. Jour. Per. Soc. Psych. 36, 929–940CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  471. McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. (1981): An interactive activation model of the effect of context in perception. Part I. An account of basic findings. Psych. Rev. 88, 375–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  472. McCloskey, D. I. (1981): Corollary discharges: Motor commands and perception. In: Handbook of physiology. sec. 1. The nervous system. vol. 2. Motor control. Brooks, V. B. (ed.). Bethesda, MD: American Physiol. SocietyGoogle Scholar
  473. McCloskey, M. E., Glucksberg, S. (1978): Natural categories: Well-defined or fuzzy sets? Memory and Cognition 6, 462–472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  474. McCloskey, M. E., Glucksberg, S. (1979): Decision processes in verifying category membership statements: Implications for models of semantic memory. Cog. Psych. 11, 1–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  475. McCloskey, M., Wible, C. G., Cohen, N. J. (1988): Is there a special flashbulb-memory mechanism? Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 171–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  476. McCulloch, W. S., Pitts, W. (1943): A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bull. Math. Biophys. 5, 115–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  477. McEntee, W. J., Mair, R. G. (1984): Some behavioral consequences of neurochemical deficits in Korsakoff psychosis. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  478. McGaugh, J. L. (1981): Behaviorism in psychology and biology. In: Encyclopedia of the twentieth century. Cappeletti, V., (ed.). Rome, Istituto Deila Enciclopedia ItalianaGoogle Scholar
  479. McGaugh, J. L. (1983): Hormonal influences on memory. Ann. Rev. Psych. 34, 297–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  480. McGaugh, J. L. (1989): Involvement of hormonal and neuromodulatory systems in the regulation of memory storage. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 255–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  481. McGaugh, J. L., Herz, M. J. (1972): Memory consolidation. San Francisco, CA: Albion Publishing CoGoogle Scholar
  482. McGaugh, J. L., Liang, K. C, Bennett, C, Sternberg, D. B. (1984): Adrenergic influences on memory storage: Interaction of peripheral and central systems. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  483. McGinty, D., Szymusiak, R. (1988): Neuronal unit activity patterns in behaving animals: Brainstem and limbic systems. Ann. Rev. Psych. 39, 135–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  484. McKoon, G., Ratcliff, R., Dell, G. (1986): A critical evaluation of the semantic-episodic distinction. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 12, 295–306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  485. McNaughton, B. L. (1983): Activity dependent modulation of hippocampal synaptic efficacy: Some implications for memory processes. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  486. Medin, D. L. (1983): Structural principles in categorization. In: Perception, cognition and development Tighe, T. J., Shepp, B. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  487. Medin, D. L., Altom, M. W., Murphy, T. D. (1984): Given versus induced category representations: Use of prototype and exemplar information in classification. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 10, 333–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  488. Medin, D. L., Schaffer, M. M. (1978): Context theory of classification in learning. Psych. Rev. 85, 207–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  489. Medin, D. L., Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1981): Linear separability in classification learning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Human Learning and Memory 7, 353–368Google Scholar
  490. Medin, D. L., Smith, E. E. (1984): Concepts and concept formation. Ann. Rev. Psych. 35, 113–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  491. Medin, D. L., Wattenmaker, W. D., Hampson, S. E. (1987): Family resemblance conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction. Cog. Psych. 19: 242–279Google Scholar
  492. Mercier, P., Baker, A. G. (1985): Latent inhibition, habituation, and sensory preconditioning: A test of priming in short-term memory. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animai Behav. Processes 11, 485–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  493. Mervis, C. B., Rosch, E. (1981): Categorization of natural objects. Ann. Rev. Psych. 32, 89–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  494. Meyers, J. L. (1976): Probability learning and sequence learning. In: Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (vol 3) Estes, W. K. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  495. Miles, F. A., Evarts, E. V. (1979): Concepts of motor organization. Ann. Rev. Psych. 30, 327–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  496. Miller, R. R. (1982): Behavioral constraints on biochemical and physiological models of memory. In: Changing concepts of the nervous system. Morrison, A. R., Strick, P. L. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  497. Miller, R. R., Marlin, N. A. (1984): The physiology and semantics of consolidation. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  498. Miller, R. R., Schachtman, T. R. (1985a): The several roles of context at the time of retrieval. In: Context and learning. Balsam, P. D., Tomie, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  499. Miller, R. R., Schachtman, T. R. (1985b): Conditioning context as an associative baseline: Implications for response generalization and nature of conditioned inhibition. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  500. Milner, B. (1970): Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In: Biology of memory. Pribram, K. H., Broadbent, D. E. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  501. Minsky, M. (1963): Steps toward artificial intelligence. In: Computers and thought. Feigenbaum, E. A., Feldman, J. (eds.). New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  502. Minsky, M., Papert, S. (1972): Perceptrons. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  503. Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R., Lewis, D. J. (1968): Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science 160, 554–555CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  504. Mishkin, M. (1982): A memory system in the monkey. Phil Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 298, 85–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  505. Mishkin, M., Aggleton, J. (1981): Multiple functional contributions of the amygdala in the monkey. In: The amygdaloid complex. Ben-Aui, Y. (ed.). New York: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
  506. Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., Bachevalier, J. (1984): Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  507. Mishkin, M., Petri, H. L. (1984): Memories and habits: Some implications for the analysis of learning and retention. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  508. Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., Macko, K. A. (1983): Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience 6, 414–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  509. Miyake, S., Fukushima, K. (1984): A neural network model for the mechanism of feature-extraction. Biol. Cyb er. 50, 377–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  510. Miyake, S., Fukushima, K. (1986): A neural network model for the mechanism of pattern information processing. In: Neural networks for computing. Denker, J. S. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  511. Miyake, S., Fukushima, K. (1989): Self-organizing neural networks with the mechanism of feedback information processing. In: Dynamic interactions in neural networks: Models and data. Arbib, M. A., Amari, S. (eds.). New York NY: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  512. Miyashita, Y. (1989): Neuronal representation of pictorial working memory in the primate temporal cortex. In: Dynamic interactions in neural networks: Models and data. Arbib, M. A., Amari, S. (eds.). New York NY: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  513. Mogenson, G. J. (1987): Limbic-motor integration. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology vol. 12 Epstein, A. N., Morrison, A. R. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  514. Moore, J. W., Solomon, P. R. (1984): Forebrain-brain stem interaction: Conditioning and the hippocampus. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R. Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  515. Moore, J. W., Stickney, K. J. (1980): Formation of attentional-associative networks in real time: Role of the hippocampus and implications for conditioning. Physiol. Psych. 8, 207–217Google Scholar
  516. Moore, J. W., Stickney, K. J. (1982): Goal tracking in attentional-associative networks: Spatial learning and the hippocampus. Physiol. Psych. 10, 202–208Google Scholar
  517. Moore, J. W., Stickney, K. J. (1985): Antiassociations: Conditioned inhibition in attentional-associative networks. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  518. Moran, J., Desimone, R. (1985): Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229, 782–784CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  519. Morris, R. G. M. (1984): Neural subsystems of exploration in rats. In: Exploration. Archer, J., Birke, L. I. A. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: Van Nostrand Reinhold (UK)Google Scholar
  520. Morris, R., Baker, M. (1984): Does long-term potentiation/ synaptic enhancement have anything to do with learning or memory. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  521. Moscovitch, M. (1985): Memory from infancy to old age: Implications for theories of normal and pathological memory. Ann. NY Acad. Sei. 444, 78–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  522. Mountcastle, V. B. (1978): An organizing principle for cerebral function: The unit module and distributed system. In: The mindful brain. Edelman, G. M., Mountcastle, V. B. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  523. Mountcastle, V. B. (1979): An organizing principle for cerebral function: The unit module and distributed system. In: The neurosciences. Schmitt, F. O., Worden, F. G. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  524. Movshon, J. A., Van Sluyters, R. C. (1981): Visual neural development. Ann. Rev. Psych. 32, 477–522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  525. Mowrer, O. H. (1947): On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of “conditioning” and problem solving. Harv. Educ. Rev. 17, 102–148Google Scholar
  526. Mowrer, O. H. (1960a): Learning theory and behavior. New York: WileyCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  527. Mowrer, O. H. (1960b): Learning theory and the symbolic processes. New York: WileyCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  528. Muenzinger, K. F. (1938): Vicarious trial and error at a point of choice: I. A general survey of its relation to learning efficiency. Jour. Genet. Psych. 53, 75–86Google Scholar
  529. Muenzinger, K. F., Fletcher, F. M. (1936): Motivation in learning VI. Escape from electric shock compared with hunger-food-tension in the visual discrimination habit. Jour. Comp. Psych. 22, 79–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  530. Muroga, S. (1971): Threshold logic and its applications. New York: John WileyGoogle Scholar
  531. Muroga, S. (1979): Logic design and switching theory. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  532. Murphy, G. L., Medin, D. L. (1985): The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psych. Rev. 92, 289–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  533. Nadel, L., Willner, J. (1980): Context and conditioning. A place for space. Physiol. Psych. 8, 218–228Google Scholar
  534. Nadel, L., Willner, J., Kurz, E. M. (1985): Cognitive maps and environmental context. In: Context and learning. Balsam, P. D, Tomie, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  535. Nadel, L., Zola-Morgan, S. (1984): Infantile amnesia: A neurobiological perspective. In: Advances in the study of communication and affect. vol 9. Moscovitch, M. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  536. Nairne, J. S., Rescorla, R. A. (1981): Second-order conditioning with diffuse auditory reinforcers in the pigeon. Learning and Motivation 12, 65–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  537. Nauta, W. J. H., Domesick, V. B. (1982): Neural associations of the limbic system. In: The neural basis of behavior. Beckman, A. L. (ed.). New York: S P Medical and Scientific BooksGoogle Scholar
  538. Neisser, U. (1967): Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  539. Neisser, U., Weene, P. (1962): Hierarchies in concept attainment. Jour. Exp. Psych. 64, 640–645CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  540. Nelson, T. O. (1976): Reinforcement and human memory. In: Handbook of learning and cognitive processes. Estes, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  541. Neves, D. M., Anderson, J. R. (1981): Knowledge compilation: Mechanisms for the automatization of cognitive skills. In: Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Anderson, J. R. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  542. Newell, A., Simon, H. A. (1972): Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-HallGoogle Scholar
  543. Nilsson, N. J. (1965): Learning machines. New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  544. Nilsson, N. J. (1980): Principles of artificial intelligence. Palo Alto, CA: Tioga PublishingGoogle Scholar
  545. Nosofsky, R. M. (1984): Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 10, 104–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  546. Nosofsky, R. M. (1986): Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 115, 39–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  547. Nosofsky, R. M. (1988a): On exemplar-based exemplar representations: Reply to Ennis (1988) Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 412–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  548. Nosofsky, R. M. (1988b): Similarity, frequency, and category representations. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 14, 54–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  549. Nosofsky, R. M. (1988c): Exemplar-based accounts of relations between classification, recognition, and typicality. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 14, 700–708CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  550. O’Brien, J. H., Packham, S. C, Brunnhoelzl, W. W. (1973): Features of spike train related to learning. Jour. Neurophysiol. 36, 1051–1061Google Scholar
  551. Oguztoreli, M. N. (1979): Activity analysis of neural networks. Biol. Cyber. 34, 159–169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  552. Ohlsson, S. (1987): Transfer of training in procedural learning: A matter of conjectures and refutations? In: Computational models of learning Bolc, L. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  553. O’Keefe, J. (1979): A review of the hippocampal place cells. Progress in Neurobiology 13, 419–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  554. O’Keefe, J. (1983): Spatial memory within and without the hippocampal system. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  555. O’Keefe, J., Nadel, L. (1878): The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford, Eng.: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
  556. Olds, J. (1975): Unit recording during Pavlovian conditioning. In: Brain mechanisms in mental retardation. Buchwald, N. A., Brazier, M. A. B. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  557. Olds, J. (1977): Drives and rewards: Behavioral studies of hypothalamic function. New York: Raven PressGoogle Scholar
  558. Olds, M. E., Fobes, J. L. (1981): The central basis of motivation: Intracranial self-stimulation studies. Ann. Rev. Psych. 32, 523–574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  559. Olton, D. S. (1978): Characteristics of spatial memory. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  560. Olton, D. S. (1979): Mazes, maps and memory. Amer. Psych. 34, 583–596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  561. Olton, D. S. (1983): Memory functions and the hippocampus. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  562. Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T., Handelmann, G. E. (1979): Hippocampus, space and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2, 313–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  563. Olton, D. S., Shapiro, M. L., Hulse, S. H. (1984): Working memory and serial patterns. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  564. Oppenheim, R. W. (1981): Neuronal cell death and some related phenomena during neurogenesis: A selective historical review and progress report. In: Studies in developmental neurobiology. Cowan, W. M. (ed.). New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  565. Optican, L. M., Richmond, B. J. (1987): Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. III. Information theoretic analysis. Jour. Neurophysiol 57, 147–161Google Scholar
  566. Overmier, J. B. (1979a): Punishment. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  567. Overmier, J. B. (1979b): Avoidance learning. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  568. Overmier, J. B. (1979c): Theories of instrumental learning. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  569. Overmier, J. B. (1985): Toward a reanalysis of the causal structure of the learned helplessness syndrome. In: Affect, conditioning, and cognition. Brush, F. R., Overmier, J. B. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  570. Overmier, J. B., Seligman, M. E. P. (1967): Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance learning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 63, 23–33Google Scholar
  571. Overton, K. L., Arbib, M. A. (1982): The extended branch-arrow model of the formation of retino-tectal connections. Biol. Cyber. 45, 157–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  572. Panksepp, J. (1986a): The neurochemistry of behavior. Ann. Rev. Psych. 37, 77–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  573. Panksepp, J. (1986b): The anatomy of emotions. In: Emotion: vol 3. Biological foundations. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  574. Papez, J. W. (1937): A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat. 38, 725–743Google Scholar
  575. Parker, D. B. (1986): A comparison of algorithms of neuron-like cells. In: Neural networks for computing. Denker, J. S. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  576. Pascoe, J. P., Kapp, B. S. (1985): Electrophysiological characteristics of amygdaloid central nucleus neurons during Pavlovian fear conditioning in the rabbit. Behav. Brain Res. 16, 117–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  577. Pavlov, I. P. (1927): Conditioned reflexes. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  578. Pearce, J. M. (1987): An introduction to animal cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  579. Pearce, J. M., Hall, G. (1979): Loss of associability by a compound stimulus comprising excitatory and inhibitory elements. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 5, 19–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  580. Pearce, J. M., Hall, G. (1980): A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psych. Rev. 87, 532–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  581. Pearce, J. M., Kaye, H., Hall, G. (1982a): Predictive accuracy and stimulus associability: Development of a model of Pavlovian learning. In: Quantitative analysis of behavior (vol. III). Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  582. Pearce, J. M., Nicholas, D. J., Dickinson, A. (1982b): Loss of associability by a conditioned inhibitor. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 33B, 149–162Google Scholar
  583. Peterson, G. B. (1984): How expectancies guide behavior. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  584. Pettigrew, J. D. (1985): Some constraints operating on the synaptic modifications underlying binocular competition in the developing visual cortex. In: Synaptic modification, neuron selectivity, and nervous system organization. Levy, W. B., Anderson, J. A., Lehmkuhle, S. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  585. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). (1982a): The physiological mechanisms of motivation. New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  586. Pfaff, D. W. (1982b): Motivational concepts: Definitions and distinctions. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  587. Pfaffmann, C. (1982): Taste: A model of incentive motivation. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  588. Pinter, R. B. (1985): Adaptation of spatial modulation transfer functions via nonlinear lateral inhibition. Biol. Cyber. 51, 285–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  589. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.) (1986): Emotion: theory, research and experience, vol III: Biological foundations. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  590. Poggio, T., Koch, C. (1987): Synapses that compute motion. Sci. Amer. 256, 46–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  591. Poo, M. (1985): Mobility and localization of proteins in excitable membranes. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 369–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  592. Porter, R. (1981): Internal organization of the motor cortex for input-output arrangements. In: Handbook of physiology, sec. 1. The nervous system, vol. 2. Motor control. Brooks, V. B. (ed.). Bethesda, MD: American Physiol. SocietyGoogle Scholar
  593. Posner, M. I., Keele, S. W. (1968): On the genesis of abstract ideas. Jour. Exp. Psych. 11, 353–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  594. Posner, M. I., Keele, S. W. (1970): Retention of abstract ideas. Jour. Exp. Psych. 83, 304–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  595. Powers, W. T. (1973): Behavior: The control of perception. New York: Aldine Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  596. Premack, D. (1978): On the abstractness of human concepts: Why it would be difficult to talk to a pigeon. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  597. Psaltis, D., Park, C. (1986): Nonlinear discriminant functions and associative memories. In: Neural networks for computing. Denker, J. S. (ed.). New York: American Institute of PhysicsGoogle Scholar
  598. Psaltis, D., Park, C. H., Hong, J. (1988): Higher order associative memories and their optical implementation. Neural Networks 1, 149–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  599. Psaltis, D., Venkatesh, S. S. (1988): Information storage in fully connected networks. In: Evolution, learning and cognition. Lee, Y. C. (ed.). Teaneck, NJ: World ScientificGoogle Scholar
  600. Pugh, G. E. (1977): The biological origin of human values. New York: Basic Books Inc.Google Scholar
  601. Quillian, M. R. (1967): Word concepts: a theory and simulation of some basic semantic capabilities. Behav. Sci. 12, 410–430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  602. Quillian, M. R. (1968): Semantic memory. In: Semantic information processing. Minsky, M. (ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  603. Quillian, M. R. (1969): The teachable language comprehender. Com. of the ACM 12, 459–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  604. Rashotte, M. E. (1979a): Reward training: Methods and data. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  605. Rashotte, M. E. (1979b): Reward training: Latent learning. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  606. Rashotte, M. E. (1979c): Reward training: Contrasts effects. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  607. Rashotte, M. E. (1979d): Reward training: Extinction. In: Animal learning: Survey and analysis. Bitterman, M. E., Lolordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Rashotte, M. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  608. Rashotte, M. E. (1981): Second-order autoshaping: Contributions to the research and theory of Pavlovian reinforcement by conditioned stimuli. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S., Gibbon, J. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  609. Rashotte, M. E., Griffin, R. W., Sisk, C. L. (1977): Second-order conditioning on the pigeon’s keypeck. Animal Learning and Behavior 5, 25–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  610. Ratcliff, R., McKoon, G. (1981): Does activation really spread? Psych. Rev. 88, 454–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  611. Razran, G. (1965): Empirical codifications and specific theoretical implications of compound-stimulus conditioning: perception. In: Classical conditioning. Prokasy, W. F. (ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  612. Reed, S. K. (1972): Pattern recognition and categorization. Cog. Psych. 3, 382–407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  613. Reed, S. K. (1973): Psychological processes in pattern recognition. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  614. Reilly, D. L., Cooper, L. N., Elbaum, C. (1982): A neural model for category learning. Biol. Cyber. 45, 35–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  615. Reiss, S., Wagner, A. R. (1972): CS habituation produces a “latent inhibition” effect but no active “conditioned inhibition”. Learning and Motivation 3, 237–245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  616. Rescorla, R. A. (1966): Predictability and number of pairing in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Psychon. Sci. 4, 382–384Google Scholar
  617. Rescorla, R. A. (1967): Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psych. Rev. 74, 71–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  618. Rescorla, R. A. (1968): Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 66, 1–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  619. Rescorla, R. A. (1969): Conditioned inhibition of fear. In: Fundamental issues in associative learning. Honig, W. K., Mackintosh, N. J. (eds.). Halifax, Can.: Dalhouseie Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  620. Rescorla, R. A. (1971): Summation and retardation tests of latent inhibition. Jour. Comp. Physiol Psych. 75, 77–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  621. Rescorla, R. A. (1972a): Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 6. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  622. Rescorla, R. A. (1972b): “Configurai” conditioning in discrete-trial bar pressing. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 79, 307–317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  623. Rescorla, R. A. (1973a): Evidence for a “unique stimulus” account of configurai conditioning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 85, 331–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  624. Rescorla, R. A. (1973b): Effect of US habituation following conditioning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 82, 137–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  625. Rescorla, R. A. (1973c): Second-order conditioning: Implications for theories of learning. In: Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning. McGuigan, F. L., Lumsden, D. B. (eds.). Washington, DC: WinstonGoogle Scholar
  626. Rescorla, R. A. (1977): Pavlovian second-order conditioning: Some implications for instrumental behavior. In: Operant Pavlovian interactions. Davis, H., Hurwitz, H. M. B. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  627. Rescorla, R. A. (1978): Some implications of a cognitive perspective on Pavlovian conditioning. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  628. Rescorla, R. A. (1979a): Aspects of the reinforcer learned in second-order Pavlovian conditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 5, 79–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  629. Rescorla, R. A. (1979b): Conditioned inhibition and extinction. In: Mechanisms of learning and motivation. Dickinson, A., Boakes, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  630. Rescorla, R. A. (1980a): Simultaneous and successive associations in sensory preconditioning. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 6, 207–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  631. Rescorla, R. A. (1980b): Pavlovian second-order conditioning: Studies in associative learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  632. Rescorla, R. A. (1981a): Simultaneous associations. In: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Harzern, P., Zeiler, M. D. (eds.). New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  633. Rescorla, R. A. (1981b): Within-signal learning in autoshaping. Animal Learning and Behavior 9: 245–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  634. Rescorla, R. A. (1982a): Simultaneous second-order conditioning produces S-S learning in conditioned suppression. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 8, 23–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  635. Rescorla, R. A. (1982b): Effect of a stimulus intervening between CS and US in autoshaping. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 8, 131–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  636. Rescorla, R. A. (1982c): Some consequences of associations between the excitor and the inhibitor in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 8, 288–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  637. Rescorla, R. A. (1982d): Comments on a technique for assessing associative learning. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior: Acquisition (v.3) Wagner, A. R., Herrnstein, R., Commons, M. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  638. Rescorla, R. A. (1983): Effect of separate presentation of the elements on within-compound learning in autoshaping. Animal Learning and Behavior 11, 439–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  639. Rescorla, R. A. (1984): Comments on three Pavlovian paradigms. In: Primary neural substrates on learning and behavioral change. Alkon, D. L., Farley, J. (eds.). New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  640. Rescorla, R. A. (1985a): Associationism in animal learning. In: Perspectives on learning and memory. Nilsson, L., Archer, T. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  641. Rescorla, R. A. (1985b): Conditioned inhibition and facilitation. In: Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition. Miller, R. R., Spear, N. E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  642. Rescorla, R. A. (1985c): Associative learning: Some consequences of contiguity. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  643. Rescorla, R. A. (1986a): Extinction of facilitation. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 12, 16–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  644. Rescorla, R. A. (1986b): Facilitation and excitation. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 12, 325–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  645. Rescorla, R. A. (1987a): A Pavlovian analysis of goal-directed behavior. Amer. Psych. 42, 119–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  646. Rescorla, R. A. (1987b): Facilitation and inhibition. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 13, 250–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  647. Rescorla, R. A., Colwill, R. M. (1983): Within-compound associations in unblocking. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 9, 390–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  648. Rescorla, R. A., Cunningham, C. L. (1978): Within-compound flavor associations. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 4, 267–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  649. Rescorla, R. A., Durlach, P. J. (1981): Within-event learning in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  650. Rescorla, R. A., Durlach, P. J., Grau, J. W. (1985a): Contextual learning in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Context and learning. Balsam, P. D., Tomie, A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  651. Rescorla, R. A., Grau, J. W., Durlach, P. J. (1985b): Analysis of the unique cue in configurai discriminations. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 356–366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  652. Rescorla, R. A., Holland, P. C. (1982): Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Ann. Rev. Psych. 33, 265–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  653. Rescorla, R. A., Solomon, R. L. (1967): Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psych. Rev. 74, 151–182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  654. Rescorla, R. A., Wagner, A. R. (1972): A theory of Pavlovian conditioning. In: Classical conditioning II. Black, A. H., Prokasy, W. F. (eds.). New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  655. Restle, F. (1957): Discrimination of cues in mazes: A resolution of the “place-vs.-response” question. Psych. Rev. 64, 217–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  656. Revusky, S., Garcia, J. (1970): Learned associations over long delays. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 4. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  657. Riccio, D. C., Ebner, D. L. (1981): Postacquisition modification of memory. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  658. Richardson-Klavehn, A., Bjork, R. A. (1988): Measures of memory. Ann. Rev. Psych. 39, 475–543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  659. Richmond, B. J., Optican, L. M. (1987): Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex II. Quantification of response waveform. Jour. Neurophysiol. 57, 147–161Google Scholar
  660. Richmond, B. J., Optican, L. M., Podell, M., Spitzer, H. (1987): Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. I. Response characteristics. Jour. Neurophysiol. 57, 132–146Google Scholar
  661. Riley, D. A., Cook, R. G., Lamb, M. R. (1981): A classification and analysis of short-term retention codes in the pigeon. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 15. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  662. Rizley, R. C., Rescorla, R. A. (1972): Associations in second-order conditioning and sensory preconditioning. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 81, 1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  663. Robbins, D., Barresi, J., Compton, P., Furst, A., Russo, M., Smith, M. A. (1978): The genesis and use of exemplar vs. prototype knowledge in abstract category learning. Memory and Cognition 6, 473–480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  664. Roberts, A., Roberts, B. L. (eds.) (1983): Neural origin of rhythmic movements. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  665. Roberts, W. A., Grant, D. S. (1976): Studies in short-term memory in the pigeon using the delayed matching-to-sample procedure. In: Processes of animal memory. Medin, D. L., Roberts, W. A., Davis, R. T. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  666. Robinson, D. A. (1981): The use of control systems analysis in the neurophysiology of eye movements. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 463–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  667. Rockel, A. J., Hiorns, R. W., Powell, T. P. S. (1980): The basic uniformity in structure of the neocortex. Brain 103, 221–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  668. Rogawski, M. A., Aghajanian, G. K. (1980): Modulation of lateral geniculate neuron excitability by noradrenaline microiontophoresis or locus ceruleus stimulation. Nature 287, 731–734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  669. Rolls, E. T. (1975): The brain and reward. New York: Pergamon PressGoogle Scholar
  670. Rolls, E. T. (1981): Processing beyond the inferior temporal visual cortex related to feeding, memory and striatal function. In: Brain mechanisms of sensation. Katsuki, Y., Norgren, R., Sato, M. (eds.). New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  671. Rolls, E. T. (1982a): Neuronal mechanisms underlying the formation and disconnection of associations between visual stimuli and reinforcement in primates. In: Advances in behavioral biology, vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  672. Rolls, E. T. (1982b): Feeding and reward. In: The neural basis of feeding and reward. Hoebel, B. G., Novin, D. (eds.). Brunswick, Main: The Haer institute for electrophysiological research.Google Scholar
  673. Rolls, E. T. (1984): Neurophysiological investigations of different types of memory in the primate. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  674. Rolls, E. T. (1986): Neural systems involved in emotion in primates. In: Emotion: vol 3. Biological foundations. Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  675. Rolls, E. T., Burton, M. J., Mora, F. (1976): Hypothalamic neuronal responses associated with the sight of food. Brain Res. 111, 53–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  676. Rolls, E. T., Bortan, H. J., Mora, F. (1980): Neurophysiological analysis of brain-simulation reward in the monkey. Brain Res. 194, 339–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  677. Roper, T. J. (1983): Learning as a biological phenomenon. In: Animal behavior vol. 3. Halliday, T. R., Slater, P. J. B. (eds.). New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  678. Rosch, E. (1978): Principles of categorization. In: Cognition and categorization. Rosch, E., Lloyd, B. B. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  679. Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B. (1975): Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cog. Psych. 7, 573–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  680. Rosenbaum, D. A. (1984): Planning and control of motor movements. In: Tutorials in learning and memory. Anderson, J. R., Kosslyn, S. M. (eds.). New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  681. Rosenblatt, F. (1958): The perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psych. Rev. 65, 386–408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  682. Rosenblatt, F. (1962): Principles of neuro dynamics: Perceptrons and the theory of brain mechanisms. Washington, DC: Spartan BooksGoogle Scholar
  683. Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L. (1984a): Basic processes and modulatory influences in the stages of memory formation. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  684. Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L. (1984b): Studying stages of memory formation with chicks and rodents. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  685. Ross, R. T. (1983): Relationships between the determinants of performance in serial feature positive discriminations. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 9, 349–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  686. Ross, R. T. (1986): Pavlovian second-order conditioned analgesia. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 12, 32–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  687. Ross, R. T., Holland, P. C. (1981): Conditioning of simultaneous and serial feature-positive discriminations. Animal Learning and Behavior 9, 293–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  688. Ross, R. T., Lolordo, V. M. (1986): Blocking during serial feature-positive discriminations: associative versus occasion-setting functions. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animai Behav. Processes 12, 315–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  689. Ross, R. T., Lolordo, V. M. (1987): Evaluation of the relation between Pavlovian occasion-setting and instrumental discriminative stimuli: A blocking analysis. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animai Behav. Processes 13, 3–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  690. Rozsypal, A. J. (1985): Computer simulation of an ideal lateral inhibition function. Biol Cyber. 52, 15–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  691. Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., Williams, R. J. (1986a): Learning internal representations by error propagation. In: Parallel distributed processing. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  692. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. (1982): An interactive activation model of the effect of context in perception. Part II. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psych. Rev. 89, 60–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  693. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland J. L. (eds.) (1986): Parallel distributed processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  694. Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., Hinton, G. E. (1986b): Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In: Parallel distributed processing. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland J. L. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  695. Rumelhart, D. E., Zipser, D. (1985): Feature discovery by competitive learning. Cog. Sci. 9, 75–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  696. Ryle, G. (1949): The concept of mind. San Francisco, CA: HutchinsonGoogle Scholar
  697. Sahley, C. L. (1984): Behavior theory and invertebrate learning. In: The biology of learning. Marler, P., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  698. Sahley, C. L., Rudy, J. W., Gelperin, A. (1981): An analysis of associative learning in a terrestrial mollusc. Jour. Comp. Physiol. 144, 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  699. Sahley, C. L., Rudy, J. W., Gelperin, A. (1984): Associative learning in a mollusc: A comparative analysis. In: Primary neural substrates on learning and behavioral change. Alkon, D. L., Farley J. (eds.). New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  700. Salafia, W. R. (1987): Pavlovian conditioning, information processing, and the hippocampus. In: Classical conditioning (3rd ed.) Gormezano, L, Prokasy, W. F., Thompson, R. F. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  701. Samuel, A. L. (1963): Some studies in machine learning using the game of checkers. In: Computers and thought. Feigenbaum, E. A., Feldman, J. (eds.). New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
  702. Sattath, S., Tversky, A. (1987): On the relation between common and distinctive feature models. Psych. Rev. 94, 16–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  703. Schacter, D. L. (1984): Toward the multidisciplinary study of memory: Ontogeny, Phylogeny and pathology of memory systems. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  704. Schacter, D. L. (1985): Multiple forms of memory in humans and animals. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  705. Schacter, D. L. (1987): Implicit memory: History and current status. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 13, 501–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  706. Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M. (1984): Infants, amnesics, and dissociable memory systems. In: Infant memory. Moscovitch, M. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  707. Schacter, D. L., Tulving, E. (1983): Memory, amnesia, and the episodic/semantic distinction. In: Expression of knowledge. Isaacson, R. L, Spear, N. E. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  708. Schlosberg, H. (1937): The relationship between success and the laws of conditioning. Psych. Rev. 44, 379–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  709. Schmajuk, N. A. (1989): The hippocampus and the control of information storage in the brain. In: Dynamic interactions in neural networks: models and data. Arbib, M. A., Amari, S. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  710. Schmajuk, N. A., Moore, J. W. (1985): Real-time attentional models for classical conditioning and the hippocampus. Physiol Psych. 13, 278–290Google Scholar
  711. Schmidt, S. R., Bohannon III J. N. (1988): In defense of the flashbulb-memory hypothesis: A comment on McCluskey, Wible and Cohen (1988). Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 332–335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  712. Schneider, A. M., Plough, M. (1983): Electroconvulsive shock and memory. In: The physiological basis of memory. Deutsch, J. A. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  713. Schneirla, T. C. (1953): Modifiability in insect behavior. In: Insect physiology. Roeder, K. D. (ed.). New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  714. Schoenfeld, W. N., Cole, B. K., Lang, J., Mankoff, R. (1973): “Contingency” in behavior theory. In: Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning. McGuigan, F. J., Lumsden, D. B. (eds.). New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  715. Schull, J. (1979): A conditioned opponent theory of Pavlovian conditioning and habituation. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 13. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  716. Schwartz, B. (1984): Psychology of learning and behavior. New York: W. W. Norton and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  717. Schwartzkroin, P. A., Taube, J. S. (1986): Mechanisms underlying long-term potentiation. In: Neural mechanisms of conditioning. Alkon, D. L., Woody, C. D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  718. Scott, G. K., Platt, J. R. (1985): Model of response-reinforcer contingency. Jour. Exp. Psych.: Animal Behav. Processes 11, 152–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  719. Sebestyen, G. S. (1962): Decision-making processes in pattern recognition. New York: The Macmillan CompanyGoogle Scholar
  720. Seibert, M., Waxman, A. M. (1989): Spreading activation layers, visual saccades, and invariant representations for neural pattern recognition systems. Neural Networks 2, 9–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  721. Seidel, R. J. (1959): A review of sensory preconditioning. Psych. Bull. 56, 58–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  722. Sejnowski, T. J. (1981): Skeleton filters in the brain. In: Parallel models of associative memory. Hinton, G. E., Anderson, J. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  723. Sejnowski, T. J. (1986): Open questions about computation in cerebral cortex. In: Parallel distributed processing, vol. 2. McClelland J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  724. Selfridge, O. (1959): Pandemonium, a paradigm for learning. In: Proc. symp. on the mechanization of thought processes. Blake, D., Uttley, A. (eds.). London, H. M. Stationery officeGoogle Scholar
  725. Selfridge, O. G., Neisser, U. (1960): Pattern recognition by machine. Sci. Amer. 203, 60–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  726. Seligman, M. E. (1968): Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 66, 402–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  727. Seligman, M. E. P. (1970): On the generality of the laws of learning. Psych. Rev. 77, 406–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  728. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975): Helplessness. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  729. Seligman, M. E. P., Hager, J. L. (eds.). (1972): Biological boundaries of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  730. Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F. (1967): Failure to escape traumatic shock. Jour. Exp. Psych. 74, 1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  731. Selverston, A. I. (1980): Are central pattern generators understandable? Behav. and Brain Sci. 3, 535–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  732. Selverston, A. I. (ed.). (1985a): Model neural networks and behavior. New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  733. Selverston, A. I. (1985b): Oscillating neural networks. Ann. Rev. Physiol. 47, 29–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  734. Selverston, A. I. (1988): A consideration of invertebrate central pattern generators as computational data bases. Neural Networks 1, 109–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  735. Shallice, T. (1982): Specific impairments of planning. Phil Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 298, 199–209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  736. Shanks, D. R. (1985): Forward and backward blocking in human contingency judgement. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 37B, 1–21Google Scholar
  737. Shanks, D. R. (1986): Selective attribution and the judgement of causality. Learning and Motivation 17, 311–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  738. Shanks, D. R. (1987): Acquisition functions in contingency judgement. Learning and Motivation 18, 147–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  739. Shanks, D. R., Dickinson, A. (1987): Associative accounts of causality judgement. In: The psychology of learning and motivation. vol. 21. Bower, G. H. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  740. Shepard, R. N. (1987): Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237, 1317–1323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  741. Shepard, R. N. (1988): Time and distance in generalization and discrimination: Reply to Ennis (1988) Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 117, 415–416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  742. Shepherd, G. M. (1979): The synaptic organization of the brain. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  743. Shepherd, G. M. (1988): Neurobiology. New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  744. Shettleworth, S. J. (1972): Constraints on learning. In: Advances in the study on behavior IV. Hinde, R. A., Shaw, E. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  745. Shettleworth, S. J. (1983): Function and mechanism in learning. In: Advances in the analysis of behavior: Biological factors in learning. Zeiler, M., Harzern, P. (eds.). London: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  746. Shortliffe, E. H., Buchanan, B. G. (1975): A model of inexact reasoning in medicine. Math. Biosci. 23, 351–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  747. Siegel, S. (1972): Latent inhibition and eyelid conditioning. In: Classical conditioning II. Black, A. H., Prokasy, W. F. (eds.). New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
  748. Silverman, R. H., Noetzel, A. S. (1988): Time-sequential self-organization of hierarchical neural networks. In: Neural information processing systems. Anderson, D. Z. (ed.). New York: American Institute of physicsGoogle Scholar
  749. Singer, W. (1984): Learning to see: Mechanisms in experience-dependent development. In: The biology of learning. Marler, P., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  750. Singer, W. (1985): Hebbian modification of synaptic transmission as a common mechanism in experience-dependent maturation of cortical functions. In: Synaptic modification, neuron selectivity, and nervous system organization. Levy, W. B., Anderson, J. A., Lehmkuhle, S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  751. Smith, E. E., Medin, D. L. (1981): Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  752. Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., Rips, L. J. (1974): Structure and process in semantic memory: A feature model for semantic decisions. Psych. Rev. 81, 214–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  753. Smith, G. P. (1982a): Satiety and the problem of motivation. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  754. Smith, G. P. (1982b): Eliot Stellar and the physiological psychology of satiety. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  755. Smith, O. A., DeVito, J. L. (1984): Central neural integration for the control of autonomic responses associated with emotion. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 42–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  756. Sokolov, E. N. (1960): Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In: The central nervous system and behavior. Bazier, M. A. B. (ed.). New York: Josiah Macy Jr. FoundationGoogle Scholar
  757. Sokolov, E. N. (1963): The orienting reflex. Ann. Rev. Psych. 25, 545–580Google Scholar
  758. Sokolov, E. N., Pakula, A., Arakelov, G. G. (1970): The aftereffects due to an intracellular electric stimulation of the giant neuron “A” in the left parietal ganglion of the mollusk Limnaea stagnalis. In: Biology of memory. Pribram, K. H., Broadbent D. E. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  759. Solomon, P. R. (1977): Role of the hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Jour. Comp. Physiol Psych. 91, 407–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  760. Solomon, P. R. (1979): Temporal versus spatial information processing theories of hippocampal function. Psych. Bull 86, 1272–1279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  761. Solomon, P. R. (1987): Neural and behavioral mechanism involved in learning to ignore irrelevant stimuli. In: Classical conditioning (3rd ed.). Gormezano, I., Prokasy, W. F., Thompson, R. F. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  762. Solomon, P. R., Moore, J. W. (1975): Latent inhibition and stimulus generalization of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response in rabbits following dorsal hippocampal ablation. Jour. Comp. Physiol Psych. 89, 1192–1203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  763. Solomon, R. L. (1982): The opponent processes in acquired motivation. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  764. Solomon, R. L., Corbit, J. D. (1974): An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psych. Rev. 81, 119–145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  765. Spear, N. E. (1981): Extending the domain of memory retrieval. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Spear, N. E., Miller, R. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  766. Spear, N. E., Mueller, C. W. (1984): Consolidation as a function of retrieval. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  767. Speers, M. J., Gillan, D. J., Rescorla, R. A. (1980): Within-compound associations in a variety of compound conditioning procedures. Learning and Motivation 11, 135–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  768. Spence, C. D., Pearson, J. C., Gelfand, J. J., Peterson, R. M. (1989): Neuronal maps for sensory-motor control in the barn owl. In: Neural information processing systems I. Touretzky, D. S. (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  769. Spence, K. W. (1947): The role of secondary reinforcement in delayed reward learning. Psych. Rev. 54, 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  770. Spence, K. W. (1956): Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CN: Yale Univ. PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  771. Spencer, W. A., Thompson, R. F., Neilson, D. R. Jr. (1966): Decrement of ventral root electronic and intracellularly recorded PSPs produced by interated cutaneous afferent volleys. Jour. Neurophysiol 29, 253–274Google Scholar
  772. Spinelli, D. N., Hirsch, H. V. B., Phelps, R. W., Metzler, J. (1972): Visual experience as a determinant of the response characteristics of cortical receptive fields in cats. Exp. Brain Res. 15, 289–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  773. Spinelli, D. N., Jensen, F. E. (1979): Plasticity: The mirror of experience. Science 203, 75–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  774. Spinelli, D. N., Jensen, F. E. (1982): Plasticity, experience and resource allocation in motor cortex and hypothalamus. In: Advances in behavioral biology. vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  775. Squire, L. R. (1982): The neuropsychology of human memory. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 241–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  776. Squire, L. R. (1983): The hippocampus and the neuropsychology of memory. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  777. Squire, L. R. (1987): Memory and brain New York: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  778. Squire, L. R., Cohen, N. J. (1984): Human memory and amnesia. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  779. Squire, L. R., Cohen, N. J., Nadel, L. (1984): The medial temporal region and memory consolidation: A new hypothesis. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  780. Staddon, J. E. R. (1983): Adaptive behavior and learning. New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  781. Standing, L. (1973): Learning 10,000 pictures. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 25, 207–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  782. Standing, L., Conezio, J., Haber, R. N. (1970): Perception and memory for pictures: Single-trial learning of 2560 visual stimuli. Psychon. Sci. 18, 89–90Google Scholar
  783. Staubli, U., Baudry, M., Lynch, G. (1985a): Olfactory discrimination learning is blocked by leupeptin, a thiol protease inhibitor. Brain Res. 337, 333–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  784. Staubli, U., Faraday, R., Lynch, G. (1985b): Pharmacological dissociation of memory: Anisomycin, a protein synthesis inhibitor, and leupeptin, a protease inhibitor, block different learning tasks. Behav. and Neural Bio. 43, 287–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  785. Stein, B. E. (1984): Development of the superior colliculus. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 95–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  786. Stein, L., Belluzzi, J. D. (1988): Operant conditioning of individual neurons. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior VII. Commons, M. L., Church, R. M., Stellar, J. R., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Hillsdale, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  787. Stellar, E. (1954): The physiology of motivation. Psych. Rev. 61, 5–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  788. Stellar, E. (1982): Brain mechanisms in hedonic processes. In: The physiological mechanisms of motivation. Pfaff, D. W. (ed.). New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  789. Stellar, J. R., Stellar, E. (1985): The neurobiology of motivation and reward. New York: Springer-VerlagGoogle Scholar
  790. Stent, G. S. (1973): A psychological mechanism for Hebb’s postulate of learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 70, 997–1001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  791. Strange, W., Kenney, T., Kessel, F., Jenkins, J. (1970): Abstraction over time of prototypes from distortions of random dot patterns. Jour. Exp. Psych. 83, 508–510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  792. Strehler, B. L., Lestienne, R. (1986): Evidence on precise time-coded symbols and memory of patterns in monkey cortical neuronal spike trains. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 83, 9812–9816CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  793. Strieker, E. M. (1983): Brain neurochemistry and the control of food intake. In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology. vol. 6. motivation. New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  794. Stuss, D. T., Benson, D. F. (1986): The frontal lobes. New York: Raven PressGoogle Scholar
  795. Suga, N. (1977): Amplitude spectrum representation in the doppler-shifter-CF processing area of the auditory cortex of the must ached bat. Science 196, 64–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  796. Suga, N., Manabe, T. (1982): Neural basis of amplitude-spectrum representation in auditory cortex of the must ached bat. Jour. Neurophysiol. 47, 225–255Google Scholar
  797. Sutton, R. S. (1984): Temporal credit assignment in reinforcement learning. Comp. and Info. Sci. Dept. U. of Mass. Amherst, Mass. tech report 84-02 Google Scholar
  798. Sutton, R. S. (1988): Learning to predict by the methods of temporal differences. Machine Learning 3, 9–44Google Scholar
  799. Sutton, R. S., Barto, A. G. (1981): Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: Expectation and prediction. Psych. Rev. 88, 135–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  800. Swanson, L. W. (1983): The hippocampus and the concept of the limbic system. In: Neurobiology of the hippocampus. Seifert, W. (ed.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  801. Tarpy, R. M. (1982): Principles of animal learning and motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  802. Teas, D. C. (1989): Auditory physiology: Present trends. Ann. Rev. Psych. 40, 405–429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  803. Terrace, H. S. (1983): Simultaneous chaining: the problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior, discrimination processes. Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J., Wagner, A. R. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: BallingerGoogle Scholar
  804. Tesauro, G. (1986): Simple neural models of classical conditioning. Biol. Cyber. 55, 187–200Google Scholar
  805. Tesauro, G. (1989): Connectionist learning of expert preferences by comparison training. In: Advances in neural information processing I. Touretzky, D. S. (ed.). San Mateo, CA: Morgen KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  806. Teyler, T. J., DiScenna, P. (1984): Long-term potentiation as a candidate mnemonic device. Brain Res. Rev. 7, 15–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  807. Teyler, T. J., DiScenna, P. (1987): Long-term potentiation. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 10: 131–161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  808. Thistlethwaite, D. (1951): A critical review of latent learning and related experiments. Psych. Bull. 48, 97–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  809. Thompson, R. F. (1972): Sensory preconditioning. In: Topics in learning and performance. Thompson, R. F., Voss, J. F. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  810. Thompson, R. F. (1985): The brain. New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  811. Thompson, R. F. (1989): Neural circuit for classical conditioning of the eyelid closure response. In: Neural models of plasticity. Byrne, J. H., Berry, W. O. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  812. Thompson, R. F., Barchas, J. D., Clark, G. A., Donegan, N., Kettner, R. E., Lavond, D. G., Madden IV, J., Mauk, M. D., McCormick, D. A. (1984c): Neuronal substrates of associative learning in the mammalian brain. In: Primary neural substrates of associative learning in the mammalian brain. Alkon, D., Farley, J. (eds.). New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  813. Thompson, R. F., Berger, T. W., Madden IV, J. (1983a): Cellular processes of learning and memory in the mammalian CNS. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 447–491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  814. Thompson, R. F., Clark, G. A., Donegan, N. H., Lavond, D. G., Lincoln, J. S., Madden IV, J., Mamounas, L. A., Mauk, M. D., McCormick, D. A., Thompson, J. K. (1984a): Neural substrates of learning and memory: A “multiple-trace” view. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  815. Thompson, R. F., Clark, G. A., Donegan, N. H., Lavond, D. G., Lincoln, J. S., Madden IV, J., Mamounas, L. A., Mauk, M. D., McCormick, D. A. (1984b): Neural substrates of basic associative learning. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  816. Thompson, R. F., Donegan, N. H., Clark, G., Lavond, D. G., Lincoln, J. S., Madden IV, J., Mamounas, L. A., Mauk, M. D., McCormick, D. A. (1987): Neuronal substrates of discrete, defensive conditioned reflexes, conditioned fear states, and their interactions in the rabbit. In: Classical conditioning (3rd ed.). Gormezano, I., Prokasy, W. F., Thompson, R. F. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  817. Thompson, R. F., McCormick, D. A., Lavond, D. G., Clark, G. A., Kettner, R. E., Mauk, M. D. (1983b): The engram found? Initial localization of the memory trace for a basic form of associative memory. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology, vol. 10. Sprague, J. M., Epstein, A. N. (eds.). New York: Academic pressGoogle Scholar
  818. Thompson, R. F., Spencer, W. A. (1966): Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psych. Rev. 173, 16–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  819. Thorndike, E. L. (1913): The psychology of learning. New York: Columbia Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  820. Thorndyke, P. W. (1984): Applications of schema theory in cognitive research. In: Tutorials in learning and memory. Anderson, J. R., Kosslyn, S. M. (eds.). New York: W. H. Freeman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
  821. Thorpe, S. J., Rolls, E. T., Maddison, S. (1983): Neuronal activity in the orbito-frontal cortex of the behaving monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 49, 93–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  822. Toates, F. (1975): Control theory in biology and experimental psychology. London, Eng: Hutchinson EducationalGoogle Scholar
  823. Toates, F. M. (1984): Exploration as a motivational and learning system: A cognitive incentive view. In: Exploration. Archer, J., Birke, L. I. A. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: Van Nostrand Reinhold (UK)Google Scholar
  824. Tolman, E. C. (1925): Behaviorism and purpose. Jour. Phil. 22, 36–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  825. Tolman, E. C. (1932): Purposive behavior in animals and man. New York: CenturyGoogle Scholar
  826. Tolman, E. C. (1933): Sign-gestalt or conditioned reflex? Psych. Rev. 40, 391–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  827. Tolman, E. C. (1934): Backward elimination of errors in two successive discrimination habits. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psych. 6, 145–152Google Scholar
  828. Tolman, E. C. (1938): The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psych. Rev. 45, 228–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  829. Tolman, E. C. (1949): There is more than one kind of learning. Psych. Rev. 56, 144–155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  830. Tolman, E. C, Brunswick, E. (1935): The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psych. Rev. 42, 43–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  831. Tolman, E. C, Gleitman, H. (1949): Studies in learning and motivation: 1. equal reinforcements in both end-boxes, followed by shock in one end-box. Jour. Exp. Psych. 39, 810–819CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  832. Tolman, E. C, Honzik, C. H. (1930a): “Insight” in rats. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psych. 4, 215–232Google Scholar
  833. Tolman, E. C, Honzik, C. H. (1930b): Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance in rats. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psych. 4, 257–275Google Scholar
  834. Tolman, E. C, Richie, B. F., Kalish, D. (1946): Studies in spatial learning: II. Place learning versus response learning. Jour. Exp. Psych. 35, 221–229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  835. Tominaga, H. (1986): Intelligent robot navigation: A simplified approach. internal report EE dept. UCI Google Scholar
  836. Touretzky, D. S. (1989): Advances in neural information processing systems I. San Mateo, CA: Morgan KaufmanGoogle Scholar
  837. Trapold, M. A. (1970): Are expectancies based upon different positive reinforcing events discriminably different? Learning and Motivation 1, 129–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  838. Treisman, A. M., Gelade, G. (1980): A feature-integration theory of attention. Cog. Psych. 12, 97–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  839. Treisman, A., Gormican, S. (1988): Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psych. Rev. 95, 15–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  840. Treisman, A., Schmidt, H. (1982): Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cog. Psych. 14, 107–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  841. Treisman, A., Southern, J. (1985): Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for for preattentive processing of separable features. Jour. Exp. Psych.: General 114, 285–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  842. Treisman, A., Sykes, M., Gelade, G. (1977): Selective attention and stimulus integration. In: Attention and performance. Dornic, S. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  843. Tsukahara, N. (1981): Synaptic plasticity in the mammalian central nervous system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 351–379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  844. Tsukahara, N. (1984): Classical conditioning mediated by the red nucleus: An approach beginning at the cellular level. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  845. Tucker, D. M., Williamson, P. A. (1984): Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psych. Rev. 91, 185–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  846. Tulving, E. (1972): Episodic and semantic memory. In: Organization of memory. Tulving, E., Donaldson, W. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  847. Tulving, E. (1983): Elements of episodic memory. London, Eng: Oxford Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  848. Tulving, E. (1984): Precis of elements of of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7, 223–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  849. Tulving, E. (1986): What kind of a hypothesis is the distinction between episodic and semantic memory? Jour. Exp. Psych.: Learning, Memory and Cognition 12, 307–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  850. Tunturi, A. R. (1952): A difference in the representation of auditory signals for the left and right ears in the isofrequency contours of right middle ecosylvian auditory cortex in the dog. Amer. Jour. Physiol. 168, 712–727Google Scholar
  851. Tversky, A. (1977): Features of similarity. Psych. Rev. 84, 327–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  852. Tversky, A., Gati, I. (1982): Similarity, separability, and the triangle inequality. Psych. Rev. 89, 123–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  853. Tversky, A., Hutchinson, J. W. (1986): Nearest neighbor analysis of psychological spaces. Psych. Rev. 93, 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  854. Udin, S. B., Fawcett, J. W. (1988): Formation of topographic maps. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 289–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  855. Ullman, S. (1986): Artificial intelligence and the brain: Computational studies of the visual system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  856. Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M. (1982): Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of visual behavior. Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A., Manfield, J. J. W. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  857. Uttley, A. M. (1979): Information transmission in the nervous system. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  858. Van Essen, D. (1985): Functional organization of primate visual cortex. In: Cerebral cortex v.S. Peters, A., Jones, E. G. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  859. Vinogradova, O. S. (1975): Functional organization of the limbic system in the process of registration of information: Facts and hypotheses. In: The hippocampus. Isaacson, R. L., Pribram, K. H. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  860. Vizi, E. S. (1984): Non-synaptic interactions between neurons: modulation of neurochemical transmission. New York: John-Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  861. Volper, D. J., Hampson, S. E. (1986): Connectionistic models of Boolean category representation. Biol. Cyber. 54, 393–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  862. Volper, D. J., Hampson, S. E. (1987): Learning and using specific instances. Biol. Cyber. 57, 57–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  863. Volper, D. J., Hampson, S. E. (in press): Quadratic function nodes: Use structure and training. Neural Networks Google Scholar
  864. von der Malsburg, C. (1973): Self-organizing of orientation sensitive cells in the striate cortex. Kybernetik 14, 85–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  865. Voronin, L. L. (1974): A study of neurophysiological mechanisms of learning on a simple behavioral model. Proc. Int. Union Physiolog. Sci. 10, 79–80Google Scholar
  866. Voronin, L. L., Gerstein, G., Kudriashov, I. E., Ioffe, S. V. (1975): Elaboration of a conditioned reflex in a single experiment with simultaneous recording of neural activity. Brain Res. 92, 383–403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  867. Wagner, A. R. (1976): Priming in STM: An information processing mechanism for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. In: Habituation: perspectives from child development, animal behavior and neurophysiology. Tighe, T. J., Leaton, R. N. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  868. Wagner, A. R. (1978): Expectancies and the priming of STM. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., Honig, W. K. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  869. Wagner, A. R. (1979): Habituation and memory. In: Mechanisms of learning and motivation. Dickinson, A., Boakes, R. A. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  870. Walters, E. T., Carew, T. J., Kandel, E. R. (1981): Associative learning in Aplysia: Evidence for conditioned fear in an invertebrate. Science 211, 504–506CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  871. Waltz, D., Feldman, J. A. (1988): Connectionist models and their implications. Norwood, NJ: Ablex PublishingGoogle Scholar
  872. Wattenmaker, W. D., Dewey, G. I., Murphy, T. D., Medin, D. L. (1986): Linear separability and concept learning: context, relational properties, and concept naturalness. Cog. Psych. 18, 158–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  873. Weinberger, N. M. (1982): Effects of conditioned arousal on the auditory system. In: The neural basis of behavior. Beckman, A. (ed.). New York: Spectrum PublishingGoogle Scholar
  874. Weinberger, N.M. (1984): The neurophysiology of learning: A view from the sensory side. In: Neuropsychology of memory. Squire, L. R., Butters, N. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  875. Weinberger, N. M., Diamond, D. M. (1987): Physiological plasticity in auditory cortex: Rapid induction by learning. Progress in Neurobiology 29, 1–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  876. Weinberger, N. M., Diamond, D. M., McKenna, T. M. (1984): Initial events in conditioning: Plasticity in the pupillomotor and auditory systems. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  877. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (eds.). (1984a): Memory consolidation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  878. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (1984b): Memory consolidation: A cognitive perspective. In: Memory consolidation. Weingartner, H., Parker, E. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  879. Weiskrantz, L. (1956): Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 49, 381–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  880. Weisman, R. G., Konigslow, R. (1984): Order competencies in animals: Models for the delayed sequence discrimination task. In: Animal cognition. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., Terrace, H. S. (eds.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  881. Weiss, K. R., Brown, B. (1974): Latent inhibition: A review and new hypothesis. Acata Neurobiologica Experimentalis 34, 301–316Google Scholar
  882. Wenzel, J., Matthies, H. (1985): Morphological changes in the hippocampal formation accompanying memory formation and long-term potentiation. In: Memory systems of the brain. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., Lynch, G. (eds.). New York: The Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  883. Wheatley, M. D. (1944): The hypothalamus and affective behavior in cats. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry 52, 296–316Google Scholar
  884. White, E. L. (1981): Thalamocortical synaptic relations. In: The organization of the cerebral cortex. Schmitt, F. O., Worden, F. G., Adelman, G., Dennis, S. G. (eds.). Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  885. Wickelgren, W. A. (1979): Chunking and consolidation. Psych. Rev. 86, 44–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  886. Wickens, D. D. (1938): The transference of conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition from one muscle group to the antagonistic muscle group. Jour. Exp. Psych. 22, 101–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  887. Widrow, B., Gupta, N. K., Maitra, S. (1973): Punish/reward: Learning with a critic in adaptive threshold systems. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cyber. 3, 455–465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  888. Widrow, B., Hoff, M. E. (1960): Adaptive switching circuits. Institute of radio engineers, western electronic show and convention. Convention Record Part I, 96–104Google Scholar
  889. Wike, E. L. (1966): Secondary reinforcement. New York: Harper and RowGoogle Scholar
  890. Williams, R. W., Herrup, K. (1988): The control of neuron number. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 423–453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  891. Wilson, B., Mackintosh, N. J., Boakes, R. A. (1985a): Matching and oddity learning in the pigeon: transfer effects and the absence of relational learning. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 37B, 295–311Google Scholar
  892. Wilson, B., Mackintosh, N. J., Boakes, R. A. (1985b): Transfer of relational rules in matching and oddity learning by pigeons and corvids. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psych. 37B, 313–332Google Scholar
  893. Winograd, T. (1975): Frame representations and the declarative-procedural controversy. In: Representation and understanding. Bobrow, D. G., Collins, A. (eds.). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  894. Wittgenstein, L. (1953): Philosophical investigations. New York: MacmillanGoogle Scholar
  895. Wong-Riley, M. (1978): Reciprocal connections between striate and prestriate cortex in squirrel monkey as demonstrated by combined peroxidas histochemistry and autoradiography. Brain Res. 147, 159–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  896. Wood, F., Ebert, V., Kinsbourne, M. (1982): The episodic-semantic memory distinction in memory and amnesia: clinical and experimental observations. In: Human memory and amnesia. Cermak, L. S. (ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
  897. Woodward, D. J., Moises, H. C, Waterhouse, B. D., Hoffer, B. J., Freeman, R. (1979): Modulatory actions of norepinephrine in the central nervous system. Fed. Proceedings 38, 2109–2116Google Scholar
  898. Woody, C. D. (1982a): Memory, learning and higher mental function. New York: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  899. Woody, C. D. (1982b): Neurophysiological correlates of latent facilitation. In: Advances in behavioral biology, vol. 26. Woody, C. D. (ed.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  900. Woody, C. D. (1984a): Studies of Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning in awake cats. In: Neurobiology of learning and memory. Lynch, G., McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M. (eds.). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
  901. Woody, C. D. (1984b): The electrical excitability of nerve cells as an index of learned behavior. In: Primary neural substrates of learning and behavioral change. Alkon, D., Farley, J. (eds.). New York: Cambridge Univ. PressGoogle Scholar
  902. Woody, C. D. (1986): Understanding the cellular basis of memory and learning. Ann. Rev. Psych. 37, 433–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  903. Woody, C. D., Berthier, N. E., Kim, E. H.-J. (1986): Rapid conditioning of an eye blink reflex in cats. In: Neural mechanisms of conditioning. Alkon, D. L., Woody, C. D. (eds.). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  904. Woody, C. D., Buerger, A. A., Ungar, R. A., Levine, D. S. (1976): Modeling aspects of learning by altering biophysical properties of a simulated neuron. Biol. Cyber. 23, 73–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  905. Woody, C. D., Kim, E. H.-J., Berthier, N. E. (1983): Effects of hypothalamic stimulation on unit responses recorded from neurons of sensorimotor cortex of awake cats during conditioning. Jour. Neurophysiol. 49, 780–791Google Scholar
  906. Wooldridge, D. E. (1979): Sensory processing in the brain. New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
  907. Zimmer-Hart, C. L., Rescorla, R. A. (1974): Extinction of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Jour. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 86, 837–845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  908. Zipser, D. (1985): A computational model of hippocampus place-fields. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 1006–1018CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Birkhäuser Boston 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Steven E. Hampson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Information & Computer ScienceUniversity of CaliforniaIrvineUSA

Personalised recommendations