Advertisement

Acalculia: Multiplication Fact Retrieval in Normal and Impaired Subjects

  • Helen J. Kahn
Part of the Human Neuropsychology book series (HN)

Abstract

Psychologists such as Binet and Galton were among the first scholars to characterize the range of arithmetic ability among individuals (Fancher, 1979). Galton focused on measurement issues and the mechanisms by which heredity and cultural factors influenced the ability to handle numbers (Galton,1908). Binet, in contrast, developed instruments based on group norms that measured intellectual abilities, including numerical aptitude, so that predictions could be generated on the capability of children to succeed in school. Binet along with Simon published the first scale of intelligence in 1905. Their test, of course, is the predecessor for the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test, still in use today.

Keywords

Multiplication Table Verification Task Correct Product Clinical Neuropsychology Math Fact 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Annett, M., Kilshaw D. (1982). Mathematical ability and lateral asymmetry. Cortex, 18, 547–568.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ashcraft, M.H., Battaglia, J. (1978). Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 527–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ashcraft, M.H., Stazyk, E.H. (1981). Mental addition: A test of three verification models. Memory and Cognition, 9, 185–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Benson, D.F., Denckla, M.B. (1969). Verbal paraphasia as a cause of calculation disturbances. Archives of Neurology, 21, 96–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Benson, D.F., Weir, W.F. (1972). Acalculia: Acquired anarithmetia. Cortex, 8, 465–472.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Boller, F., Grafman, J. (1983). Acalculia: Historical development and current significance. Brain and Cognition, 2, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Campbell, J.I.D. (1987). Network interference and mental multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 109–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, A.M., Loftus, E.F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Coltheart, M. (1984). Editorial. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dahman, W., Hartje, W. Bussing, A., Strum.W. (1982). Disorders of calculation in aphasic patients-spatial and verbal components. Neuropsychologia, 20, 145–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Deloche, G., Seron, X. (1982a). From one to 1: An analysis of a transcoding process by means of neuropsychological data. Cognition, 12, 119–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Deloche, G., Seron, X. (1982b). From three to 3: A differential analysis of skills in transcoding quantities between patients with Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain, 105, 719–733.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fancher, R.E. (1979). Pioneers of Psychology. New York: W.W. Norton, Company.Google Scholar
  14. Galion, F. (1908) Memories of My Life. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  15. Geary, D.C., Widaman, K.F., Little, T.D. (1986). Cognitive addition and multiplication: Evidence for a single memory network. Memory and Cognition, 14, 478–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grafman, J., Passafiume, D., Faglioni, P., Boller, F. (1982). Calculation disturbances in adults with focal hemispheric damage. Cortex, 18, 37–50.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Grewel, F. (1969). The acalculias. In P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam: North-Holland, Vol. 4, 181–196.Google Scholar
  18. Grewel, F. (1952). Acalculia. Brain, 75, 397–407.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Groen, G.J., Parkman, J.M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition. Psychological Review, 79, 329–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hecaen, H., Angelergues, R., Houillier. S. (1961). Les varietes cliniques des acalculies au cours des lesions retrorolandiques: Approche statisque du probleme. Revue Neurologique, 105, 85–103.Google Scholar
  21. Henschen, S.E. (1926). On the function of the right hemisphere of the brain in relation to the left in speech, music and calculation. Brain, 49, 110–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jackson, M., Warrington, E.K. (1986). Arithmetic skills in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Cortex, 22, 611–620.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kahn, H.J. (1986). Individual differences in cognitive processes: Calculation and semantic memory. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, N.D.Google Scholar
  24. Kahn, H.J., Konewko, P. (1986). An exam for acalculia.Google Scholar
  25. Katz, A.N. (1980). Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for right hemispheric mediation in an elementary component stage. Quarterly Journal of Psychology, 32, 69–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Levin, H.S. (1979). The acalculias. In K.M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (Eds.) Clinical Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press. 128–140.Google Scholar
  27. McCloskey, M. Caramazza, A., Basili, A. (1985). Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation. Evidence from dyscalculia. Brain and Cognition, 4, 171–196.Google Scholar
  28. McCloskey, M., Sokol, S., Goodman, A. (1986). Cognitive processes in verbal-number production: Inferences from the performance of brain-damaged subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 307–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Parkman, J.M. (1972). Temporal aspects of simple multiplication and comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95, 437–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Parkman, J.M., Groen, G.J. (1971). Temporal aspects of simple addition and comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 335–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Seron, X., Deloche, G. (1984). From 2 to two: An analysis of a transcoding process by means of neuropsychological evidence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13, 215–236.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Seron, X., Deloche, G. (1983). From 4 to four: A supplement to “From three to 3”. Brain, 106, 735–744.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Singer, H.D., Low, A.A. (1933). Acalculia (Henschen): A clinical study. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 29, 476–498.Google Scholar
  34. Spellacy, F., Peter, B. (1978). Dyscalculia and elements of the developmental Gerstmann Syndrome in school children. Cortex, 14, 197–206.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Stazyk, E.H., Ashcraft, M.H., Hamann, M.S. (1982). A network approach to mental multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 320–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Warrington, E.K. (1982). The fractionation of arithmetical skills: A single case study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 31–51.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Helen J. Kahn

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations