Cause and Function in the Development of Behavior Systems

  • Jerry A. Hogan
Part of the Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology book series (HBNE, volume 9)


The purpose of this chapter is to present a general framework for studying the development of behavior. Kruijt (1964) proposed that, in young animals, the motor components of behavior often function as independent units, and that only later, often after specific experience, do these motor components become integrated into more complex systems, such as hunger, aggression, and sex. The thesis to be defended here is a generalization of this proposal: The building blocks of behavior are various kinds of perceptual, motor, and central components, all of which can exist independently. The study of development is primarily the study of changes in these components themselves and in the connections among them.


Sexual Behavior Animal Behaviour Behavior System Central Mechanism Zebra Finch 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Adler, N. T., and Hogan, J. A. Classical conditioning and punishment of an instinctive response in Betta splendens. Animal Behaviour, 1963, 11, 351–354.Google Scholar
  2. Andrew, R. J. Precocious adult behaviour in the young chick. Animal Behaviour, 1966, 14, 485–500.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baerends, G. P. An evaluation of the conflict hypothesis as an explanatory principle for the evolution of displays. In G. P. Baerends, C. Beer, and A. Manning (Eds.), Function and evolution in behaviour. London: Oxford University Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  4. Baerends, G. P. The functional organization of behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 1976, 24, 726–738.Google Scholar
  5. Baerends, G. P., and Kruijt, J. P. Stimulus selection. In R. A. Hinde and J. G. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. London: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  6. Baerends-van Roon, J. M., and Baerends, G. P. The morphogenesis of the behaviour of the domestic cat, with a special emphasis on the development of prey-catching. Verhandelingen der Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Welenschappen, Afd. Natuurkunde, Tweede Reeks (Proceedings of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences, Section Physics, Second Series), Part 72, 1979.Google Scholar
  7. Baptista, L. F., and Petrinovich, L. Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song template hypothesis in the white-crowned sparrow. Animal Behaviour, 1984, 32, 172–181.Google Scholar
  8. Baylis, J. R. Avian vocal mimicry: Its function and evolution. In D. E. Kroodsma, E. H. Miller, and H. Ouellet (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
  9. Beach, F. A. Analysis of factors involved in the arousal, maintenance, and manifestation of sexual excitement in male animals. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1942, 4, 173–198.Google Scholar
  10. Bekoff, A., and Kauer, J. A. Neural control of hatching: Role of neck position in turning on hatching leg movements in post-hatching chicks. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 1982, 145, 497–504.Google Scholar
  11. Blass, E. M., Hall, W. G., and Teicher, M. H. The ontogeny of suckling and ingestive behaviors. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 1979, 8, 243–299.Google Scholar
  12. Blass, E. M., Ganchrow, J. R., and Steiner, J. E. Classical conditioning in newborn humans 2–48 hours of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 1984, 7, 125–134.Google Scholar
  13. Boakes, R., and Panter, D. Secondary imprinting in the domestic chick blocked by previous exposure to a live hen. Animal Behaviour, 1985, 33, 353–365.Google Scholar
  14. Bolhuis, J. J., and Trooster, W. J. Reversibility revisited: Stimulus-dependent stability of filial preference in the chick. Animal Behaviour, 1988, 36.Google Scholar
  15. Bolhuis, J. J., Johnson, M., and Horn, G. Effects of early experience on the development of filial preferences in the domestic chick. Developmental Psychobiology, 1985, 18, 299–308.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bottjer, S. W., and Arnold, A. P. The ontogeny of vocal learning in songbirds. In E. M. Blass (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Vol. 8. New York: Plenum Press, 1986.Google Scholar
  17. Daly, M. Why don’t male mammals lactate? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 1979, 78, 325–345.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Doty, R. W. The concept of neural centers. In J. C. Fentress (Ed.), Simpler networks and behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1976.Google Scholar
  19. Drachman, D. B., and Sokoloff, L. The role of movement in embryonic joint development. Developmental Biology, 1966, 14, 401–420.Google Scholar
  20. Evans, R. M. Early aggressive responses in domestic chicks. Animal Behaviour, 1968, 16, 24–28.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Fantz, R. L. Form preferences in newly hatched chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1957, 50, 422–430.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Farris, H. E. Classical conditioning of courting behavior in the Japanese quail (Coturnix c. japonica). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 213–217.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, D. Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 1982, 218, 179–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gallistel, C. R. The organization of action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980.Google Scholar
  25. Graf, J. S., Balsam, P. D., and Silver, R. Associative factors and the development of pecking in the ring dove. Developmental Psychobiology, 1985, 18, 447–460.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Groothuis, A. G. G. The ontogeny of complex species-specific motor patterns, studied in gull displays. Abstracts of the 19th International Ethological Conference, Toulouse, 1985.Google Scholar
  27. Guiton, P. The influence of imprinting on the agonistic and courtship responses of the brown leghorn cock. Animal Behaviour, 1961, 9, 167–177.Google Scholar
  28. Hale, C., and Green, L. Effect of initial pecking consequences on subsequent pecking in young chicks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1979, 93, 730–735.Google Scholar
  29. Hall, W. G., and Browde, J. A. The ontogeny of independent ingestion in mice: Or, why won’t infant mice feed? Developmental Psychobiology, 1986, 19, 211–222.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hall, W. G., and Williams, C. L. Suckling isn’t feeding, or is it? A search for developmental continuities. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1983, 13, 219–254.Google Scholar
  31. Hamburger, V. Anatomical and physiological basis of embryonic motility in birds and mammals. In G. Gottlieb (Ed.), Behavioral embryology, New York: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  32. Harlow, H. F., and Harlow, M. K. Social deprivation in monkeys. Scientific American, Nov. 1962.Google Scholar
  33. Harlow, H. F., and Harlow, M. K. The affectional systems. In A. M. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, and F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1965.Google Scholar
  34. Harlow, H. F., and Suomi, S. J. Social recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1971, 68, 1534–1538.Google Scholar
  35. Hebb, D. O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949.Google Scholar
  36. Hess, E. H. Natural preferences of chicks and ducklings for objects of different colors. Psychological Reports, 1956, 2, 477–483.Google Scholar
  37. Hinde, R. A. Animal behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1970.Google Scholar
  38. Hinde, R. A. Biological bases of human social behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1974.Google Scholar
  39. Hogan, J. A. An experimental study of conflict and fear: An analysis of behavior of young chicks to a mealworm. Part I. The behavior of chicks which do not eat the mealworm. Behaviour, 1965, 25, 45–97.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hogan, J. A. An experimental study of conflict and fear: An analysis of behavior of young chicks to a mealworm. Part II. The behavior of chicks which eat the mealworm. Behavior, 1966, 27, 273–289.Google Scholar
  41. Hogan, J. A. The development of a hunger system in young chicks. Behaviour, 1971, 39, 128–201.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Hogan, J. A. Development of food recognition in young chicks. I. Maturation and nutrition. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1973a, 83, 355–366.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Hogan, J. A. How young chicks learn to recognize food. In R. A. Hinde and J. G. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. London: Academic Press, 1973b.Google Scholar
  44. Hogan, J. A. Responses in Pavlovian conditioning studies. Science, 1974, 186, 156–157.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Hogan, J. A. The ontogeny of food preferences in chicks and other animals. In L. M. Barker, M. Best, and M. Domjan (Eds.), Learning mechanisms in food selection. Waco, TX: Baylor University Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  46. Hogan, J. A. An eccentric view of development: A review of The Dynamics of Behavior Development by Zing-Yang Kuo. Contemporary Psychology, 1978, 23, 690–691.Google Scholar
  47. Hogan, J. A. Hierarchy and behavior: A review of The Organization of Action by C. R. Gallistel. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1981, 4, 625.Google Scholar
  48. Hogan, J. A. Cause, function, and the analysis of behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 1984a, 10, 65–71.Google Scholar
  49. Hogan, J. A. Pecking and feeding in chicks. Learning and Motivation, 1984b, 15, 360–376.Google Scholar
  50. Hogan, J. A. The structure versus the provenance of behavior: A comment on B. F. Skinner’s “Ontogeny and phylogeny of behavior.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1984c, 7, 690.Google Scholar
  51. Hogan, J. A., and Roper, T. J. A comparison of the properties of different reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1978, 8, 155–255.Google Scholar
  52. Hogan-Warburg, A. J., and Hogan, J. A. Feeding strategies in the development of food recognition in young chicks. Animal Behaviour, 1981, 29, 143–154.Google Scholar
  53. Hollis, K. The biological function of Pavlovian conditioning: The best defense is a good offense. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1984, 10, 413–425.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Horn, G. Memory, imprinting, and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1985.Google Scholar
  55. Johnson, M. H., Bolhuis, J. J., and Horn, G. Interaction between acquired preferences and developing predispositions during imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 1985, 33, 1000–1006.Google Scholar
  56. Johnston, T. D., and Gottlieb, G. Deyelopment of visual species identification in ducklings: What is the role of imprinting? Animal Behaviour, 1981, 29, 1082–1099.Google Scholar
  57. Johnston, T. D., and Gottlieb, G. Effects of social experience on visually imprinted maternal preferences in Peking ducklings. Developmental Psychobiology, 1985, 18, 261–271.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Kenny, J. T., Stoloff, M. L., Bruno, J. P., and Blass, E. M. The ontogeny of preferences for nutritive over nonnutritive suckling in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1979, 93, 752–759.Google Scholar
  59. Konishi, M. The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalizations in the white-crowned sparrow. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1965, 22, 770–783.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Kruijt, J. P. Imprinting in relation to drive interactions in Burmese red junglefowl. Symposia of the Zoological Society, London, 1962, 8, 219–226.Google Scholar
  61. Kruijt, J. P. Ontogeny of social behaviour in Burmese red junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour, 1964, Supplement 9.Google Scholar
  62. Kruijt, J. P. On the development of social attachments in birds. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 1985, 35, 45–62.Google Scholar
  63. Kuo, Z. Y. The dynamics of behavioral development. New York: Random House, 1967.Google Scholar
  64. Lehrman, D. S. Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, and J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior. San Francisco: Freeman, 1970.Google Scholar
  65. Logan, C. A. Biological diversity in avian vocal learning. In M. D. Zeiler and P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behavior, Vol. 3: Biological factors in learning. Chichester, England: Wiley, 1983.Google Scholar
  66. Lorenz, K. Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal far Ornithologie, 1935, 83, 137–213, 289–413.Google Scholar
  67. Lorenz, K. Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften, 1937, 25, 289–300, 307–318, 324–331.Google Scholar
  68. Lorenz, K. Plays and vacuum activities. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l’homme. Paris: Masson et Cie, 1956.Google Scholar
  69. Lorenz, K. Phylogenetische Anpassung und adaptive Modifikation des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift fiir Tierpsychologie, 1961, 18, 139–187.Google Scholar
  70. Lorenz, K. Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1965.Google Scholar
  71. Marier, P. A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology (monograph supplement), 1970, 71, 1–25.Google Scholar
  72. Marier, P. Sensory templates in species-specific behavior. In J. C. Fentress (Ed.), Simpler networks and behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer, 1976.Google Scholar
  73. Marier, P. Song learning: Innate species differences in the learning process. In P. Marler and H. S. Terrace (Eds.), The biology of learning, Dahlem Workshop Reports—Life Sciences Research Report 29. Berlin: Springer, 1984.Google Scholar
  74. Marier, P., and Peters, S. Subsong and plastic song: Their role in the vocal learning process. In D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
  75. Martin, P., and Caro, T. M. On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1985, 15, 59–103.Google Scholar
  76. Maynard Smith, J. Parental investment: A prospective analysis. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 1–9.Google Scholar
  77. McGregor, P. K., and Avery, M. I. The unsung songs of great tits (Parus major): Learning neighbors’ songs for discrimination. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 1986, 18, 311–316.Google Scholar
  78. Milligan, M. M., and Verner, J. Inter-populational song dialect discrimination in the white-crowned sparrow. Condor, 1971, 73, 208–213.Google Scholar
  79. Moore, B. R. The role of direct Pavlovian reactions in simple instrumental learning in the pigeon. In R. A. Hinde and J. G. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. London: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  80. Morgan, C. L. Habit and instinct. London: Arnold, 1896.Google Scholar
  81. Morris, D. [Discussion following Lorenz] In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l’homme. Paris: Masson et Cie, 1956, pp. 642–643.Google Scholar
  82. Nottebohm, F. A brain for all seasons: Cyclical anatomical changes in song control nuclei of the canary brain. Science, 1981, 214, 1368–1370.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Novak, M. and Harlow, H. F. Social recovery of monkeys isolated for the first year of life: I. Rehabilitation and therapy. Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 453–465.Google Scholar
  84. Oppenheim, R. The ontogeny of behavior in the chick embryo. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 1974, 5, 133–172.Google Scholar
  85. Park, T. J., and Dooling, R. J. Perception of species-specific contact calls by budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1985, 99, 391–402.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Paton, J. A., and Nottebohm, F. Neurons generated in the adult brain are recruited into functional circuits. Science, 1984, 225, 1046–1048.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Petrinovich, L. Factors influencing song development in the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1985, 99, 15–29.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Pinel, J. P., and Treit, D. Burying as a defensive response in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1978, 92, 708–712.Google Scholar
  89. Pinel, J. P., and Treit D. Conditioned defensive burying in rats: Availability of burying materials. Animal Learning and Behavior, 1979, 7, 392–396.Google Scholar
  90. Polsky, R. H. Hunger, prey feeding, and predatory aggression. Behavioral Biology, 1975, 13, 81–93.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Reisbick, S. H. Development of food preferences in newborn guinea pigs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1973, 85, 427–442.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Rice, J. C. Effects of learning constraints and behavioural organization on the association of vocalizations and hunger in Burmese red junglefowl chicks. Behaviour, 1978, 67, 259–298.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Rozin, P. The acquisition of food habits and preferences. In J. D. Matarazzo, S. M. Weiss, J. A. Herd, N. E. Miller, and S. M. Weiss (Eds.), Behavioral health: A handbook of health enhancement and disease prevention. New York: Wiley, 1984.Google Scholar
  94. Rozin, P., and Schiller, D. The nature and acquisition of a preference for chili pepper by humans. Motivation and Emotion, 1980, 4, 77–101.Google Scholar
  95. Sackett, G. P. Unlearned responses, differential rearing experiences, and the development of social attachments by Rhesus monkeys. In L. A. Rosenblum (Ed.), Primate behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  96. Schiller, P. H. Manipulative patterns in the chimpanzee (1949). In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior. New York: International Universities Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  97. Schneirla, T. C. Interrelationships of the “innate” and the “acquired” in instinctive behavior. In L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l’homme. Paris: Masson et Cie, 1956.Google Scholar
  98. Schneirla, T. C., Rosenblatt, J. S., and Tobach, E. Maternal behavior in the cat. In H. Rheingold (Ed.), Maternal behavior in mammals. New York: Wiley, 1963.Google Scholar
  99. Schutz, F. Sexuelle Prägung bei Anatiden. Zeitschril für Tierpsychologie, 1965, 22, 50–103.Google Scholar
  100. Sherry, D. F. Parental food-calling and the role of the young in the Burmese red junglefowl (Gallus g. spadiceus). Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 594–601.Google Scholar
  101. Sherry, D. F. Parental care and development of thermoregulation in red junglefowl. Behaviour, 1981, 76, 250–279.Google Scholar
  102. Shettleworth, S. J. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 1975, 1, 56–87.Google Scholar
  103. Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.Google Scholar
  104. Slater, P. J. B. Bird song learning: Theme and variations. In G. A. Clark, Jr., and A. R. Brush (Eds.), Perspectives in ornithology. London: Cambridge University Press, 1983.Google Scholar
  105. Steiner, J. E. Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 1979, 13, 257–295.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Steller, E. Drive and motivation. In J. Field, H. W. Magoun, and V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of physiology, Sec. 1, Vol. 3. Washington: American Physiological Association, 1960.Google Scholar
  107. Stevenson, J. G. Reinforcing effects of chaffinch song. Animal Behaviour, 1967, 15, 427–432.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Sutherland, N. S. The learning of discriminations by animals. Endeavour, 1964, 23, 148–152.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. ten Cate, C. The influence of social relations on the development of species recognition in zebra finch males. Behaviour, 1984, 91, 263–285.Google Scholar
  110. ten Cate, C. Sexual preferences in zebra finch males exposed to two species. I. A case of double imprinting. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1986, 100, 248–252.Google Scholar
  111. ten Cate, C. Sexual preferences in zebra finch males exposed to two species. II. The internal representation resulting from double imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 1987, 35, 321–330.Google Scholar
  112. Thorpe, W. H. The learning of song patterns by birds, with especial reference to the song of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Ibis, 1958, 100, 535–570.Google Scholar
  113. Thorpe, W. H. Bird song. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1961.Google Scholar
  114. Thorpe, W. H., and Jones, F. G. W. Olfactory conditioning and its relation to the problem of host selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B. 1937, 124, 56–81.Google Scholar
  115. Timberlake, W. The functional organization of appetitive behavior: Behavior systems and learning. In M. D. Zeiler and P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behavior, Vol. 3. Biological factors in learning. Chichester, England: Wiley, 1983.Google Scholar
  116. Tinbergen, N. The study of instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1951.Google Scholar
  117. Tinbergen, N. Derived activities: Their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 1952, 27, 1–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. von Holst, E. Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 1954, 2, 89–94.Google Scholar
  119. von Holst, E., and St. Paul, U. von. Vom Wirkungsgefüge der Triebe. Naturwissenschaften, 1960, 47, 409–422. (Trans.: On the functional organisation of drives. Animal Behaviour, 1963, 11, 1–20.)Google Scholar
  120. Waddington, C. H. Principles of development and differentiation. New York: Macmillan, 1966.Google Scholar
  121. Wasserman, E. A. Pavlovian conditioning with heat reinforcement produces stimulus-directed pecking in chicks. Science, 1973, 181, 875–877.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Zajonc, R. B., Wilson, W. R., and Rajecki, D. W. Affiliation and social discrimination produced by brief exposure in day-old domestic chicks. Animal Behaviour, 1975, 23, 131–138.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jerry A. Hogan
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Zoology LaboratoryUniversity of GroningenA.A. HarenThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of TorontoTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations