Advertisement

The Central Place of Imagery in Human Functioning

  • Eric Klinger

Abstract

This paper presents three broad propositions regarding imagery. First, imagery partakes of processes central to human functioning. Second, control over imagery constitutes control over a large part of a person’s total psychic apparatus. Third, intervention methods based on imagery therefore have great power. The paper is not intended as a comprehensive or critical review but rather as a broad overview intended to highlight the argument.

Keywords

Human Functioning Central Place Mental Practice McCollough Effect Mental Rehearsal 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Beasley, W. F. The effect of physical and mental practice of psychomotor skills on chemistry student laboratory performance. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, 1978.) Dissertation Abstracts International, 1979, 39 (9-A), 5428.Google Scholar
  2. Connella, J. The effects of psycho-imagination therapy on the treatment outcome of chronic benign pain patients. Imagery: Bulletin of the American Association for the Study of Mental Imagery, 1980, 3, (3), 6–7.Google Scholar
  3. Drummond, P., White, K. and Ashton, R. Imagery vividness affects habituation rate. Psychophysiology, 1978, 15, 193–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Finke, R. A. Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. Psychological Review, 1980, 87, 113–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Freud, S. Project for a scientific psychology. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 1). London: Hogarth, 1960. (First published in 1895.)Google Scholar
  6. Gibson, E. J. Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.Google Scholar
  7. Gibson, J. J. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1966.Google Scholar
  8. Hoelscher, T. J., Klinger, E., and Barta, S. G. Incorporation of concern- and nonconcern-related verbal stimuli into dream content. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
  9. Jacobson, E. The electrophysiology of mental activities. American Journal of Psychology, 1932, 44, 677–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Jeffrey, R. W. The influence of symbolic and motor rehearsal in observational learning. Journal of Research Personality, 1976, 10, 116–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Klinger, E. Modes of normal conscious flow. In K. S. Pope and J. L. Singer (Eds.) The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigations into the flow of human experience New York: Plenum, 1978.Google Scholar
  12. Leuner, H. Basic principles and therapeutic efficacy of guided affective imagery (GAI). In J. L. Singer and K. S. Pope (Eds.), The power of human imagination: New methods in psychotherapy. New York: Plenum, 1978.Google Scholar
  13. Leuner, H. Grundlinien des Katathymen Bilderleben aus neuerer Sicht. In H. Leuner (Ed.), Kathathymes Bilderleben: Ergebnisse in Theorie und Praxis Bern: Huber, 1980.Google Scholar
  14. Lipsky, M. J., Kassinove, H., & Miller, N.J. Effects of rational-emotive therapy, rational role reversal, and rational-emotive imagery on the emotional adjustment of community mental health center patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1980, 48, 366–374.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. McGuigan, F. J. Covert oral behavior during the silent performance of language tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 74, 309–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. McMahon, C. Images as motives and motivators: A historical perspective. American Journal of Psychology, 1973, 86, 465–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 1977, 198, 75–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.Google Scholar
  19. Neisser, U. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman, 1976.Google Scholar
  20. Piaget, J. Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton, 1962. (First published in 1945.)Google Scholar
  21. Rawlings, E. I., and Rawlings, I. L. Rotary pursuit tracking following mental rehearsal as a function of voluntary control of visual imagery. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1974, 38, 302.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Rawlings, E. I., Rawlings, I. L., Chen, S. S., and Yilk, M. D. The facilitating effects of mental rehearsal in the acquisition of rotary pursuit tracking. Psychonomic Science, 1972, 26, 71–73.Google Scholar
  23. Richardson, A. Mental practice: A review and discussion. I. Research Quartly, 1967, 38, 95–107. (a)Google Scholar
  24. Richardson, A. Mental practice: A review and discussion. II. Research Quartly, 1967, 38, 263–273. (b)Google Scholar
  25. Schwartz, G. E., Fair, P. L., Salt, P., Mandel, M. R., & Klerman, G. L. Facial muscle patterning to affective imagery in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Science, 1976, 192, 489–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Shorr, J. E. Psychotherapy through imagery. New York: Intercontinental Medical Book Corporation, 1974.Google Scholar
  27. Shorr, J. E. An overview of psycho-imagination therapy. In R. Corsini (Ed.), Innovative psychotherapies. New York: Wiley, in press.Google Scholar
  28. Shute, C. C. D. The McCollough effect: An indicator of central neurotransmitter activity Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
  29. Smyth, M. M. The role of mental practice in skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1975, 7, 199–206.Google Scholar
  30. Sokolov, E. N. The modeling properties of the nervous system. In M. Cole and I. Maltzman (Eds.), A handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology. New York: Basic Books, 1969.Google Scholar
  31. Start, K. B., and Richardson, A. Imagery and mental practice. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 1964, 34, 280–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Strickland, B. R., Hale, W. D., and Anderson, L. K. Effect of induced mood states on activity and self-reported affect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1975, 43, 587.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Summers, J. J. Adjustment to redundance in reaction time: A comparison of three learning methods. Acta Psychologica, 1977, 41, 205–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tomkins, S. S. Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 1: The positive affects. New York: Springer, 1962.Google Scholar
  35. Ulich, E. Some experiments on the function of mental training in the acquisition of motor skills. Ergonomics, 1967, 10, 411–419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wachter, H.-M., and Pudel, V. Kurztherapie von 15 Sitzungen mit dem Katathymen Bilderleben. In H. Leuner (Ed.), Katathymos Bilderleben: Ergebnisse in Theorie und Praxis. Bern: Huber, 1980.Google Scholar
  37. White, K. D., Ashton, R., and Lewis, S. Learning a complex skill: Effects of mental practice, physical practice, and imagery ability. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 1979, 10, 71–78.Google Scholar
  38. Wilke, E. Das Katathyme Bilderleben bei der konservativen Behandlung der Colitis ulcerosa. In H. Leuner (Ed.), Katathymes Bilderleben; Ergebnisse in Theorie und Praxis. Bern: Huber, 1980.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1981

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eric Klinger
    • 1
  1. 1.University of MinnesotaMorrisUSA

Personalised recommendations