Skip to main content
  • 162 Accesses

Abstract

The most persistent advocate of conditioning is undoubtedly E. R. Guthrie. Guthrie considers association by contiguity in time, or “simultaneous conditioning,” the most general law in psychology. “Stimuli acting at the time of a response tend on their recurrence to evoke that response,” he says. Any other type of behavior can be derived from simultaneous conditioning. Especially the processes of learning represent the general law of stimulaneous conditioning or association by contiguity in time of stimuli and responses. “The outstanding characteristics of learning which have been expressed in forms of frequency, intensity, irradiation, temporary extinction, conditioned inhibition, forgetting, forward and backward conditioning, and so on, are all derivable from this more general law,”1 Guthrie believes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Bibliography

1, 2, 3, 4, 5. Guthrie, Hull, Skinner, Tolman, Razran

  • Adams, O. K. Experimental studies of adaptive behavior in cats. Comp. Psychol Monogr., 1929, 6, No. 27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, W. J. Simple reaction chains and their integration. I. Homogeneous chaining with terminal reinforcement. J. comp., physiol. Psychol., 1947, 40, 349–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baernstein, H. D., & Hull, C. L. A mechanical model of the conditioned reflex. J. gen. Psychol., 1931, 5, 99–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bass, M. J., & Hull, C. L. The irradiation of a tactile conditioned reflex in man. J.comp. Psychol., 1934, 17, 46–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bendig, A. W. Latent learning in a water maze. J. exp. Psychol., 1953, 43, 134–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, G., & Spence, K. W. The logic of psychological measurement. Psychol. Rev., 1944, 51, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birch, H. G., & Bittermann, M. E. Reinforcement and learning: The process of sensory integration. Psychol. Rev., 1949, 56, 292–308.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blodgett, H. C. The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1929, 4, 113–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., & Farber, I. E. Emotions conceptualized as intervening variables—with suggestions toward a theory of frustration. Psychol. Bull, 1951, 48, 465–495.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buxton, C. E. Latent learning and the goal gradient hypothesis. Contr. psychol Theor., 1940, 2, No. 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm, R. M. Intentionality and the theory of signs. Philos. Stud., 1952, 3, 56–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie, R. The role of drive discrimination in learning under irrelevant motivation. J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 42, 13–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christie, R. The effect of some early experiences in the latent learning of adult rats. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 43, 281–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, S. W., & Skinner, B. F. Some factors influencing the distribution of associated words. Psychol. Rec., 1939, 3, 178–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crespi, L. P. Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. Amer. J. Psychol., 1942, 55, 467–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crespi, L. P. Amount of reinforcement and level of reinforcement. Psychol Rev., 1944, 51, 341–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daub, C. T. The effect of doors on latent learning. J. comp. Psychol., 1933, 15, 49–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny, M. R. The role of secondary reinforcement in a partial reinforcement learning situation. J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 373–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny, M. R., & David, R. H. A test of latent learning for a non-goal signifícate. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1951, 44, 590–595.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, M. H. The effect of appropriateness of rewards and of complex incentives on maze performance. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1929, 4, 91–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellson, D. G. Quantitative studies of the interaction of simple habits. I. Recovery from specific and generalized effects of extinction. J. exp. Psychol., 1938, 23, 339–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. An experimental study of punishment. Psychol Monogr., 1944, 57, No. 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. Effects of competing reactions on the conditioning curve for bar-pressing. J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 200–205.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. Learning theory and the new “mental chemistry.” Psychol Rev., 1960, 67, 207–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K., Koch, S., MacCorquodale, K., Meehl, P. E., Mueller, C. G., Jr., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Verplanck, W. S. Modern Learning Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. Some quantitative properties of anxiety. J. exp. Psychol., 1941, 29, 390–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felsinger, J. M., Gladstone, A. I., Yamaguchi, H. G., & Hull, C. L. Reaction latency (stR) as a function of the number of reinforcements (N). J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 214–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finan, J. L. Quantitative studies in motivation. I. Strength of conditioning in rats under varying degrees of hunger. J. comp. Psychol., 1940, 29, 119–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. A systematic application of the concepts of generalization and differentiation to verbal learning. Psychol Rev., 1940, 47, 196–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilchrist, J. C. Characteristics of latent and reinforcement learning as a function of time. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 198–203.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gladstone, A. I., Yamaguchi, H. G., Hull, C. L., & Felsinger, J. M. Some functional relationships of reaction potential (sEr) and related phenomena. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 510–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, H. Studies in motivation and learning: II. Thirsty rats trained in maze with food but not water; then run hungry. J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 169–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C. H., & Gagné, R. M. The acquisition, extinction, and spontaneous recovery of a conditioned operant response. J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 26, 251–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, G. R. An experimental test of the expectation theory of learning. J. comp. physiol Psychol., 1940, 41, 137–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grindley, G. C. Experiments on the influence of the amount of reward on learning in young chickens. Brit. J. Psychol., 1929, 20, 173–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Purpose and mechanism in psychology. J. Phil, 1924, 21, 673–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Conditioning as a principle of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1930, 37, 412–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. On the nature of psychological explanations. Psychol. Rev., 1933, 40, 124–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Association as a function of time interval. Psychol Rev., 1933, 40, 355–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Reward and punishment. Psychol. Rev., 1934, 41, 450–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York: Harper, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Psychological principles and scientific truth. Proc. 25th Anniv. Celeb. Inaugur. grad. Stud., University of Southern California, 1936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of human conflict. New York: Harper, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The effect of outcome on learning. Psychol. Rev., 1939, 46, 480–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Association and the law of effect. Psychol Rev., 1940, 47, 127–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Conditioning: a theory of learning in terms of stimulus, response and association. Yearb. nat. Soc. Stud. Educ, 1942, 41, 17–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The principle of associative learning. In F. P. Clarke, & M. C. Nahm (Eds.), Philosophical essays in honor of Edgar Arthur Singer, Jr. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1942.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. Psychological facts and psychological theory. Psychol. Bull., 1946, 43, 1–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The status of systematic psychology. Amer. Psychologist, 1950, 5, 97–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. (Rev. ed.) New York: Harper, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R., & Horton, G. P. Cats in a puzzle box. New York: Rinehart, 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. & Powers, F. F. Educational psychology. New York: Ronald, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haney, G. W. The effect of familiarity on maze performance of albino rats. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1931, 4, 319–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hempel, C. G. The logical analysis of psychology. In H. Feigl & W. Sellars (Eds.), Readings in philosophical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1949, pp. 373–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herb, F. H. Latent learning—non-reward followed by food in blinds. J. comp. Psychol., 1940, 29, 247–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. T. The rate of extinction in maze-bright and maze-dull rats. Psychol. Rev., 1940, 4, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. T. Internal stimuli and learning. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1949, 42, 486–492.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. T., & Skinner, B. F. Changes in hunger during starvation. Psychol Rev., 1947, 1, 51–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. & Bower, G. H. Theories of learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, C. H. Goal gradient, anticipation, and perseveration in compound trial-and-error learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1930, 25, 566–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C. I. The generalization of conditioned responses: I. The sensory generalization of conditioned responses with varying frequencies of tone. J. gen. Psychol., 1937, 17, 125–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C. I. The generalization of conditioned responses: II. The sensory generalization of conditioned responses with varying intensities of tone. J. genet. Psychol., 1937, 51, 279–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C. I. The generalization of conditioned responses. IV. The effects of varying amounts of reinforcement upon the degree of generalization of conditional responses. J. exp. Psychol., 1937, 21, 261–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. A functional interpretation of the conditioned relfex. Psychol Rev., 1929, 36, 498–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Simple trial-and-error learning: A study in psychological theory. Psychol. Rev., 1930, 37, 241–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms. Psychol. Rev., 1930, 37, 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived as habit phenomena. Psychol. Rev., 1931, 38, 487–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The goal gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psychol Rev., 1932, 39, 25–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Differential habituation to internal stimuli in the albino rat. J. comp. Psychol., 1933, 16, 255–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The concept of the habit-family hierarchy, and maze learning. Part I. Psychol. Rev., 1934, 41, 33–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The concept of the habit-family hierarchy, and maze learning. Part II. Psychol Rev., 1934, 41, 134–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Learning: II. The factor of the conditioned reflex. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbookof general experimental psychology. Worcester, Mass.: Clark University Press, 1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The rat’s speed of locomotion gradient in the approach to food. J. comp. Psychol., 1934, 17, 393–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The mechanism of the assembly of behavior segments in novel combinations suitable for problem solution. Psychol. Rev., 1935, 42, 219–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The conflicting psychologies of learning—a way out. Psychol. Rev., 1935, 42, 491–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The influence of caffeine and other factors on certain phenomena of rote learning. J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 13, 249–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Special review: Thorndike’s Fundamentals of learning. Psychol Bull, 1935, 32, 807–823.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Mind, mechanism and adaptive behavior. Psychol Rev., 1937, 44, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The goal-gradient hypothesis applied to some “field-force” problems in the behavior of young children. Psychol Rev., 1938, 45, 271–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The problem of stimulus equivalence in behavior theory. Psychol. Rev., 1939, 46, 9–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Modern behaviorism and psychoanalysis. Trans. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1939, 1, Ser. II, 78–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Simple trial-and-error learning—an empirical investigation. J. comp. Psychol., 1939, 27, 233–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Explorations in the patterning of stimuli conditioned to the G.S.R. J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 27, 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Psychological seminar memoranda, 1939–1940. Bound mimeographed manuscript on file in the libraries of Yale University, University of Iowa, & Oberlin College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Conditioning: Outline of a systematic theory of learning. Yearb. nat. Soc. Stud. Educ., 1942, 41, Part 2, 61–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The problem of intervening variables in molar behavior theory. Psychol. Rev., 1943, 50, 273–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. A postscript concerning intervening variables. Psychol. Rev., 1943, 50, 540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Psychological memoranda, 1940–1944. Bound mimeographed manuscript on file in the libraries of Yale University, University of Iowa, & University of North Carolina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The place of innate individual and species differences in a natural-science theory of behavior. Psychol Rev., 1945, 52, 55–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The discrimination of stimulus configurations and the hypothesis of afferent neural interaction. Psychol. Rev., 1945, 52, 133–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Research memorandum concerning the quantitative empirical determination of certain basic behavioral constants and their functional relationships. May 20, 1945. (Mimeographed manuscript on file in Yale Library.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The problem of primary stimulus generalization. Psychol Rev., 1947, 54, 120–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Reactively heterogeneous compound trial-and-error learning with distributed trials and terminal reinforcement. J.exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 118–135.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Reactively heterogeneous compound trial-and-error learning with distributed trials and serial reinforcement. J.exp. Psychol., 1948, 38, 17–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Memorandum on behavior theory. Oct. 4, 1949. (Mimeographed manuscript on file in Yale Library).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Stimulus intensity dynamism (V) and stimulus generalization. Psychol Rev., 1949, 56, 67–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Behavior postulates and corollaries—1949. Psychol Rev., 1950, 57, 173–180.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Simple qualitative discrimination learning. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 303–313.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. A primary social science law. Sci. Mon., N.Y. 1950, 71, 221–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Memorandum concerning behavior theory. Feb. 7, 1950. (Mimeographed manuscript on file in Yale Library.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Memorandum concerning behavior theory. Mar. 6, 1950. (Mimeographed manuscript on file in Yale Library.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Essentials of behavior. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. A Behavior System. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Clark L. Hull. In E. G. Boring, H. S. Langfeld, H. Werner, & R. M. Yerkes (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiographyVolume IV. Worcester, Mass.: Clark University Press, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., & Baernstein, H. D. A mechanical parallel to the conditioned reflex. Science, 1929, 70, 14–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., Felsinger, J. M. Gladstone, A. I., and Yamaguchi, H. G. A proposed quantification of habit strength. Psychol. Rev., 1947, 54, 237–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., Hovland, C. I., Ross, R. T., Hall, M., Perkins, D. T., & Fitch, F. B. Mathematicodeductive theory of rote learning: A study in scientific methodology. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1940.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., Johnston, R. L., Rouse, R. O., & Barker, A. H. True, sham, and esophageal feeding as reinforcements. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1951, 44, 236–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., & Krueger, R. C. An electro-chemical parallel to the conditioned reflex. J. gen. Psychol., 1931, 5, 262–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L., & Mowrer, O. H. Hull’s psychological seminars, 1936–38. Notices and abstracts of proceedings. Bound mimeographed material on file in the libraries of the University of Chicago, University of North Carolina, & Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. & Spence, K. W. “Correction” vs. “non-correction” method of trial-and-error learning in rats. J. comp. Psychol., 1938, 25, 127–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L. G. The effect of random alteration of reinforcement on the acquisition and extinction of conditioned eyelid reactions. J. esp. Psychol., 1939, 25, 141–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L. G. Generalization as a function of method of reinforcement. J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 25, 361–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, W. O., & Stanley, J. C. Partial reinforcement: A review and critique. Psychol. Bull, 1950, 47, 193–234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kappauf, W. E., & Schlosberg, H. Conditioned responses in the white rat. III. Conditioning as a function of the length of the period of delay. J. genet. Psychol., 1937, 50, 27–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karn, H. W., & Porter, H. M., Jr. The effects of certain pre-training procedures upon maze performance and their significance for the concept of latent learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 461–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H. Drive interaction: II. Experimental analysis of the role of drive in learning theory. J. exp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 188–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H. A comparison of learning under motivated and satiated conditions in the white rat. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 545–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H. An investigation of latent learning in a T-maze. J. comp. Psychol., 1947, 40, 265–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H. “What is learned?”—a theoretical blind alley. Psychol. Rev., 1952, 59, 269–277.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H., & Ianner, J. H. A further test of the ability of rats to learn the location of food when motivated by thirst. J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 762–765.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. Conditioning as a function of the time between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 1–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. An experimental test of a two-factor theory of inhibition. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 15–23.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. Foundations of conditioning and learning. New York. Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. The logical character of the motivation concept. I. Psychol. Rev., 1941, 48, 15–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. Review of Hull’s Principles of Behavior. Psychol. Bull, 1944, 41, 269–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. Form and content in hypothetico-deductive systems: A reply to Woodbury. J. Psychol., 1947, 24, 237–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. & Hull C. L. In W. K. Estes & others. (Eds.), Modem learning theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S., & Daniel, W. J. The effect of satiation on the behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength. J. exp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 167–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. New York. Oxford, 1948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krechevsky, I. “Hypotheses” versus “chance” in the presolution period in sensory discrimination-learning. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1932, 6, 27–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, R. The role of motivation in learning: A study of the phenomenon of differential motivational control of the utilization of habits. J. genet. Psychol., 1935, 46, 3–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, R. Dr. Hull’s Principles of Behavior. J. genet. Psychol., 1944, 65, 3–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, R. The experiments by Spence and Lippitt and by Kendler on the sign-gestalt theory of learning. J.exp. Psychol., 1948, 38, 102–106.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Littman, R. A. Latent learning in a T-maze after two degrees of training. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1950, 43, 135–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Littman, R. A., & Rosen, E. Molar and molecular. Psychol Rev., 1950, 57, 58–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K. Preliminary suggestions as to a formalization of expectancy theory. Psychol. Rev., 1953, 60, 55–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K., & Meehl, P. E. “Cognitive” learning in the absence of competition of incentives. J. comp.physiol Psychol., 1949, 42, 383–390.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. An examination of the treatment of stimulus patterning in Professor Hull’s Principles of Behavior. Psychol Rev., 1945, 52, 324–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. On the circularity of the law of effect. Psychol Bull, 1950, 47, 52–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. A failure to find the Blodgett effect, and some secondary observations on drive conditioning. J. comp physiol Psychol., 1951, 44, 178–183.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. Some methological comments concerning expectancy theory, Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 230–233.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. Drive conditioning as a factor in latent learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1953, 43, 20–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E., & MacCorquodale, K. A. A further study of latent learning in the T-maze, J. comp. physiol Psychol., 1948, 41, 372–396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. J. exp. Psychol., 1948, 38, 89–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Learnable drives and rewards. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Learning of visceral and glandular responses. Science, 1969, 163, 434–445.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, K. C. “Spontaneous alternation” as a function of time between trials and amount of work. J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 42, 82–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, K. C. Exploratory behavior and its relation to spontaneous alternation in a series of maze exposures. J. comp. physiol Psychol., 1952, 45, 50–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Two factor learning theory reconsidered with special reference to secondary reinforcement and the concept of habit. Psychol Rev., 1956, 63, 114–128.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H., & Jones, H. M. Extinction and behavior variability as functions of effortfulness of task. J. exp. Psychol., 1943, 33, 369–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muenzinger, K. F., Koerner, L., & Irey, E. Variability of an habitual movement in guinea pigs. J. comp. Psychol., 1929, 9, 425–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, H. W. Description of the learned response in discrimination behavior. Psychol Rev., 1950, 57, 121–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notterman, J. M. A study of some relations among aperiodic reinforcement, discrimination training, and secondary reinforcement. J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41, 161–169.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perin, C. T. Behavior potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction. J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 30, 93–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perin, C. T. A quantitative investigation of the delay-of-reinforcement gradient. J. exp. Psychol., 1943, 32, 37–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L. The history and present status of the law of effect. Psychol. Bull, 1947, 44, 489–563.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, W. C. H. Continuity in human learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 187–194.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Sensory capacities of the dog as studied by the conditioned reflex method (with C. J. Warden). Psychol Bull, 1929, 26, 202–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Theory of conditioning and of related phenomena. Psychol Rev., 1930, 37, 225–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Conditioned responses in animals other than dogs. Psychol Bull, 1933, 30, 261–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Conditioned withdrawal responses in adult human subjects. Psychol Bull, 1934, 31, 111–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Salivary conditioning in adult human subjects, Psychol Bull, 1934, 31, 634–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Conditioned responses: An experimental study and a theoretical analysis. Arch.Psychol., 1935, 28, No. 191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Psychology in the U.S.S.R. J. Phil, 1935, 32, 19–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Salivary conditioning in adult human subjects. Psychol Bull, 1935, 32, 561–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. The conditioning of voluntary reactions. J. exp. Psychol., 1936, 19, 653–655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Attitudinal control of human conditioning. J. Psychol., 1936, 2, 327–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Conditioned responses: A classified bibliography. Psychol. Bull, 1937, 34, 191–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Configurai and colligated conditioning. Psychol. Bull, 1937, 34, 515–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Semantic and phonetographic generalizations of salivary conditioning to verbal stimuli. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 642–652.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Attitudinal determinants of conditioning and of generalization of conditioning. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 820–829.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Some psychological factors in the generalization of salivary conditioning to verbal timuli. Amer. J. Psychol., 1949, 62, 247–256.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Stimulus generalization of conditioned responses. Psychol. Bull, 1949, 46, 337–365.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. The conditioned evocation of attitudes (cognitive conditioning). J. exp. Psychol., 1954, 48, 278–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Conditioning and perception. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 83–95.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Backward conditioning. Psychol. Bull, 1956, 53, 55–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Extinction re-examined and re-analyzed: A new theory. Psychol Rev., 1956, 63, 39–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. The dominance-contiguity theory of the acquisition of classical conditioning. Psychol. Bull, 1957, 54, 1–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. Pavlov and Lamarck. Science, 1958, 128, 758–760.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, B. A. The acquisition of a trace conditioned response as a function of the magnitude of the stimulus trace. J. exp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 15–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, B. A. The relationship between the strength of a habit and the degree of drive present during acquisition. J. exp. Psychol., 1949 39, 296–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, B. F. Hull’s treatment of learning. Psychol. Bull, 1944, 41, 640–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, B. F. Studies in spatial learning. III. Two paths to the same location and two paths to two different locations. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 25–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, B. F. Studies in spatial learning. VI. Place orientation and direction orientation. J. exp.Psychol., 1948, 38, 659–669.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, B. F. Theories of learning: A consumer report. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973, pp. 451–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. S. Analysis of detour behavior. I. Learning of round-about pathways in fish. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1949, 42, 463–475.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, W. N. An experimental approach to anxiety, escape and avoidance behavior. Chapter 5 in P. Hoch & J. Zubin (Eds.), Anxiety. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. S. Concepts as involving laws and inconceivable without them Phil. Sci., 1948, 15, 287–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. Mind, meaning, and behavior. Philos. Stud., 1952, 3, 83–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seward, J. P. A theoretical derivation of latent learning. Psychol. Rev., 1947, 54, 83–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seward, J. P. An experimental analysis of latent learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 177–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seward, J. P. Secondary reinforcement as tertiary motivation: A revision of Hull’s revision. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 362–374.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seward, J. P., Levy, N., & Handlon, J. P., Jr. Incidental learning in the rat. J. comp, physiol. Psychol., 1950, 43, 240–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, M. E., & Waters, R. H. An experimental test of latent learning in a relatively free-choice situation. J. genet. Psychol., 1950, 77, 283–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheffield, F. D. Avoidance training and the contiguity principle. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1948, 41, 165–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheffield, F. D., & Roby, T. B. Reward value of a non-nutritive sweet taste. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1950, 43, 471–481.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, Rietta. The relative effectiveness of certain incentives in animal learning. Comp. sychol. Monogr., 1924, 2, 1–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. On the conditions of elicitation of certain eating reflexes. Proc. nat. Acad. Sc., 930, 6, 433–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. J. gen. Psychol., 1931, 5, 27–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Drive and reflex strength. J gen. Psychol., 1932, 6, 22–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Drive and reflex strength: II. J gen. Psychol., 1932, 6, 38–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex. J. gen. Psychol., 1932, 7, 74–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. On the rate of extinction of a conditioned reflex. J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 8, 114–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The measurement of “spontaneous activity.” J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 9, 3–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The rate of establishment of a discrimination. J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 9, 302–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. “Resistance to extinction” in the process of conditioning, J. gen. Psychol., 1933, 9, 420–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The abolishment of a discrimination. Proc. nat. Acad. Sci., 1933, 19, 825–828.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The extinction of chained reflexes. Proc. nat. Acad. Sci., 1934, 20, 234–237.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A discrimination without previous conditioning. Proc. nat. Acad. Sci., 1934, 20, 532–536.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, 40–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudotype. J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, 66–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A discrimination based upon a change in the properties of a stimulus. J. gen. Psychol., 1935, 12, 313–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The extinction ratio and its modification by a temporal discrimination. Psychol. Bull., 1936, 33, 784. (Abstr.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A failure to obtain “disinhibition.” J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 14, 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The reinforcing effect of a differentiating stimulus. J. gen. Psychol., 1936 14, 263–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The effect of the amount of conditioning of an interval of time before reinforcement. J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 14, 279–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Conditioning and extinction and their relation to drive. J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 14, 296–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Thirst as an arbitrary drive. J. gen. Psychol., 1936, 15, 205–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The verbal summator and a method for the study of latent speech. J. Psychol., 1936 2, 71–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The distribution of associated words. Psychol. Rec., 1937, 1, 71–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. J. gen. Psychol., 1937, 16, 272–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The alliteration in Shakespeare’s sonnets: A study of literary behavior. Psychol. Rec., 1939, 3, 186–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A method of maintaining an arbitrary degree of hunger. J. comp. Psychol., 1940, 30, 139–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A quantitative estimate of certain types of sound patterning in poetry. Amer. J. Psychol., 1941, 54, 64–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The processes involved in the repeated guessing of alternatives. J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 30, 495–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Reply to Dr. Yacorzynsky. J. exp. Psychol., 1943, 32, 93–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Review of Hull’s Principles of Behavior. Amer. J. Psychol., 1944, 57, 276–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychol Rev., 1945, 52, 270–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Experimental psychology. In W. Dennis (Ed.), Current trends in psychology. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. “Superstition” in the pigeon. J. exp. Psychol., 1948, 38, 168–172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 193–216.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Cumulative record. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F., & Heron, W. T. Effects of caffeine and benzedrine upon conditioning and extinction. Psychol. Rec., 1937, 1, 340–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychol. Rev., 1936, 43, 427–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Analysis of the formation of visual discrimination habits in the chimpanzee. J. comp. Psychol., 1937, 23, 77–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. Psychol. Rev., 1937, 44, 430–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Continuous versus non-continuous interpretations of discrimination learning. Psychol. Rev., 1940, 47, 271–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Failure of transposition in size discrimination of chimpanzees. Amer. J. Psychol., 1941, 54, 223–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. The basis of solution by chimpanzees of the intermediate size problem. J. exp. Psychol., 1942, 31, 257–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Theories of learning. In F. A. Moss (Ed.), Comparative psychology. (Rev. ed.) New York: Prentice-Hall, 1942.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. An experimental test of the continuity and non-continuity theories of discrimination learning. J. esp. Psychol., 1945, 35, 253–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. The role of secondary reinforcement in delayed reward learning. Psychol. Rev., 1947, 54, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. The postulates and methods of “behaviorism.” Psychol. Rev., 1948, 55, 67–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 159–172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Theoretical interpretations of learning. In C. P. Stone (Ed.), Comparative psychology. (3rd ed.) New York: Prentice-Hall, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Theoretical interpretations of learning. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W., Bergmann, G., & Lippitt, R. O. A study of simple learning under irrelevant motivational-reward conditions. J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 539–551.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W., & Lippitt, R. O. “Latent” learning of a simple maze problem with relevant needs satiated. Psychol. Bull., 1940, 37, 429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W., & Lippitt, R. O. An experimental test of the sign-gestalt theory of trial and error learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 491–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprow, A. J. Reactively homogenous compound trial-and-error learning with distributed trials and terminal reinforcement. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 197–213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strange, J. R. Latent learning under conditions of high motivation. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1950, 43, 194–197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thistlethwaite, D. L. A critical review of latent learning and related experiments. Psychol. Bull., 1951, 48, 97–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thistlethwaite, D. L. An experimental test of a reinforcement interpretation of latent learning. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1951, 44, 431–441.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thistlethwaite, D. L. Conditions of irrelevant incentive learning. J. comp, physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 517–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. The hierarchical organization of nervous mechanisms underlying instinctive behavior. In Physiological mechanisms in animal behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinklepaugh, O. L. An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. J. comp. Psychol., 1928, 8, 197–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. More concerning the temporal relations of meaning and imagery. Psychol. Rev., 1917, 24, 114–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Nerve process and cognition. Psychol. Rev., 1918, 25, 423–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Retroactive inhibition as affected by conditions of learning. Psychol. Monogr., 1918, 25, No. 107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Instinct and purpose. Psychol. Rev., 1920, 27, 217–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Can instincts be given up in psychology? J. abnorm., soc. Psychol., 1922, 17, 139–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A new formula for behaviorism. Psychol. Rev., 1922, 29, 44–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A behavioristic account of the emotions. Psychol. Rev., 1923, 30, 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The effects of underlearning upon long- and short-time retentions. J. exp. Psychol., 1923, 6, 466–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The nature of instinct. Psychol. Bull., 1923, 20, 200–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The inheritance of maze-learning ability in rats. J. comp. Psychol., 1924, 4, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Behaviorism and purpose. J. Phil, 1925, 22, 36–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Purpose and cognition: The determiners of animal learning. Psychol. Rev., 1925, 32, 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A behavioristic theory of ideas. Psychol. Rev., 1926, 33, 352–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The nature of the fundamental drives. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1926, 20, 349–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A behaviorist’s definition of consciousness. Psychol. Rev., 1927, 34, 433–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Habit formation and higher mental processes in animals. Psychol. Bull., 1927, 24, 1–35;

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Habit formation and higher mental processes in animals. Psychol. Bull.,1928, 25, 24–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Purposive behavior. Psychol. Rev., 1928, 35, 524–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. “Insight” in rats. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1930, 4, 215–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Maze performance a function of motivation and of reward as well as of knowledge of the maze paths. J. gen. Psychol., 1930, 4, 338–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Purposive Behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century, 1932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The law of effect. A reply to Dr. Goodenough. J. exp. Psychol., 1933, 16, 463–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Sign-gestalt or conditioned reflex? Psychol. Rev., 1933, 40, 246–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Backward elimination of errors in two successive discrimination habits. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1934, 5, 145–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Theories of learning. In F. A. Moss (Ed.), Comparative psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1934, pp. 367–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Psychology versus immediate experience. Philos. Sci., 1935, 2, 356–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Operational behaviorism and current trends in psychology. Proc. 25th Anniv. Celeb. Inaugur. grad. Stud. Los Angeles: University of Southern California Press, 1936.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The perception of spatial relations by the rat: A type of response not easily explained by conditioning. J. comp. Psychol., 1936, 22, 287–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Demands and conflicts. Psychol Rev., 1937, 44, 158–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The acquisition of string-pulling by rats—conditioned response—or sign-gestalt? Psychol. Rev., 1937, 44, 195–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. An operational analysis of “demands.” Erkenntnis., 1937, 6, 383–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C The action of punishment in acceleration learning. J. comp. Psychol., 1938, 26, 187–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The determiners of behavior at a choice point. Psychol. Rev., 1938, 45, 1–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A reply to Professor Guthrie. Psychol. Rev., 1938, 45, 163–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The law of effect. Psychol. Rev., 1938, 45, 200–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Prediction of vicarious trial and error by means of the schematic sowbug. Psychol. Rev., 1939, 46, 318–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Spatial angle and vicarious trial and error. J. comp. Psychol., 1940, 30, 129–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Discrimination vs. learning and the schematic sowbug. Psychol. Rev., 1941, 48, 367–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Motivation, learning and adjustment. Proc. Amer. philos. soc., 1941, 84, 543–563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A drive-conversion diagram. Psychol. Rev., 1943, 50, 503–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. A stimulus-expectancy need-cathexis psychology. Science, 1945, 101, 160–166.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Studies in spatial learning. II. Place learning vs. response learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 221–229.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Studies in spatial learning. IV. The transfer of place learning to other starting paths. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 39–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Studies in spatial learning: VII. Place and response learning under different degrees of motivation. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 653–659.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. Discussion: Interrelationships between perception and personality. J. Pers., 1949, 18, 48–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The nature and functioning of wants. Psychol. Rev., 1949, 56, 357–369.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. The psychology of social learning. J. soc. Issues, 1949, 5, Suppl. No. 3, 5–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Brunswik, E. The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychol. Rev., 1935, 42, 43–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Davis, F. C. A note on the correlations between two mazes. J. comp. Psychol., 1924, 4, 125–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Geier, F. M. Goal distance and restless activity. I. The goal gradient of restless activity. J. comp. Psychol., 1943, 35, 197–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Gleitman, H. Studies in learning and motivation: I. Equal reinforcements in both end-boxes, followed by shock in one end-box. J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 810–819.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., Hall, C. S., & Bretnall, E. P. A disproof of the law of effect and a substitution of the laws of emphasis, motivation, and disruption. J. exp. Psychol., 1932, 15, 601–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. Degrees of hunger, reward and non-reward, and maze learning in rats. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1930, 4, 241–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. Introduction and removal of reward and maze performance of rats. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1930, 4, 257–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Horowitz, J. A. A reply to Mr. Koffka. Psychol. Bull., 1933, 30, 459–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. & Kalish, D. Studies in spatial learning. I. Orientation and the short-cut. J. exp. Psychol., 1946, 36, 13–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Krechevsky, I. Means-end-readiness and hypothesis. A contribution to comparative psychology. Psychol. Rev., 1933, 40, 60–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Levin, M. Individual differences in emotionality, hypothesis formation, vicarious trial and error, and visual discrimination learning in rats. Comp. Psychol. Monogr., 1941, 17, No. 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Minium, E. VTE in rats: Overlearning and difficulty of discrimination. J. comp. Psychol., 1942, 34, 301–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Ritchie, B. F. Correlation between VTE’s on a maze and on a visual discrimination apparatus. J. comp. Psychol., 1943, 36, 91–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., Ritchie, B. F., & Kalish, D. Studies in spatial learning. V. Response learning vs. place learning by the non-correction method. J. exp. Psychol., 1947, 37, 285–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Robinson, E. W. The effect of degrees of hunger upon the order of elimination of long and short blinds. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1930, 4, 189–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & Sams, C. F. Time discrimination in white rats. J. comp. Psychol., 1925, 5, 255–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., & White, A. E. A note on the elimination of short and long blind alleys. J. comp. Psychol., 1923, 3, 327–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voeks, V. W. Formalization and clarification of a theory of learning. J. Psychol., 1950, 30, 341–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L. The acquisition of a response to food under conditions of food satiation. Amer. Psychologist, 1948, 3, 239. (Reported by title only).

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L. Drive specificity and learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1948, 38, 39–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L. Drive specificity and learning: Demonstration of a response tendency acquired under a strong irrelevant drive. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1951, 44, 596–603.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L., Knotter, M. C., & DeValois, R. L. Drive specificity and learning: The acquisition of a spatial response to food under conditions of water deprivation and food satiation. J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 161–168.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, W. B., & Nolan, C. Y. Cues for discrimination as secondary reinforcing agents: A confirmation. J. comp, physiol. Psychol., 1953, 46, 180–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R. K. The case for the Tolman-Lewin interpretation of learning. Psychol Rev., 1943, 50, 157–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcoxen, H. C., Hays, R., & Jull, C. L. A preliminary determination of the functional relationship of effective reaction potential (sEr) to the ordinal number of Vincentized extinction reactions (n). J. exp. Psychol., 1950, 40, 194–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. A. The reward value of a conditioned stimulus. Univer. Calif. Publ. Psychol., 1929, 4, 31–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, S. B. Resistance to extinction as a function of the number of reinforcements. J. exp. Psychol., 1938, 23, 506–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, R. C., & Dennis, W. The dependence of the white rat’s choice of pathways upon the length of the daily trial series. J. comp. Psychol., 1934, 18, 135–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfle, H. M. Time factors in conditioning finger-withdrawal. J. gen. Psychol., 1930, 4, 372–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury, C. B. The learning of stimulus patterns by dogs. J. comp. Psychol., 1943, 35, 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, H. G. Superthreshold reaction potential (sEr) as a function of experimental extinction (n). J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41, 391–400.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, H. G. Drive (D) as a function of hours of hunger (h). J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 42, 108–117.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, H. G., Hull, C. L., Felsinger, J. M., & Gladstone, A. I. Characteristics of dispersons based on the pooled momentary reaction potentials (sEr) of a group. Psychol Rev., 1948, 55, 216–238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Youtz, R. E. P. The weakening of one Thorndikian response following the extinction of another. J. exp. Psychol., 1939, 24, 294–304.

    Google Scholar 

6. Learning Theory Influenced by Psychoanalysis

  • Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., Sears, R. R., Ford, C. S., Hovland, C. I., & Sollenberger, R. T. Frustration and aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1939.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. Defense against ego-threat in memory and perception. J. abnorm, soc. Psychol., 1942, 47, 230–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, S. M. An empirical study of the castration and oedipus complexes. Genet. Psychol Monogr., 1952, 46, 61–130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. Theories of learning. (Rev. ed.) New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R., Kubie, L. S., & Pumpian-Mindlin, E. Psychoanalysis and science. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Modern behaviorism and psychoanalysis. Trans. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1939, 1, 78–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F. Frustration, the study of behavior without a goal. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Experiments relating Freudian displacement to generalization of conditioning. Psychol. Bull., 1939, 36, 516–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. The frustration-aggression hypothesis. Psychol. Rev., 1941, 48, 337–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Experimental studies in conflict. In J. McV. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders. New York: Ronald, 1944.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. Theory and experiment relating psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-response generalization. J. abnorm., soc. Psych., 1948, 43, 155–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1941.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misbach, L. Psychoanalysis and theories of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1948, 55, 143–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Anxiety reduction and learning. J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 27, 497–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. An experimental analogue of “repression” with incidental observations on “reaction formation.” J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1940, 35, 56–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. On the dual nature of learning: a re-interpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.” Harvard educ. Rev., 1947, 17, 102–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and the neurotic paradox. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 1948, 18, 577–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and personality dynamics. New York: Ronald, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Two-factor learning theory: Summary and comment. Psychol. Rev., 1951, 58, 350–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Psychotherapy: Theory and research. New York: Ronald, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory: historical review and re-interpretation. Harvard educ. Rev., 1954, 24, 37–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, E. J., & Berkum, M. M. Displacement as a function of conflict. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1955, 51, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarbin, T. R. Mental age changes in experimental regression. J. Pers., 1950, 19, 221–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Experimental studies of projection: I. Attribution of traits. J. soc. Psychol., 1936, 7, 151–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Experimental studies of projection: II. Ideas of reference. J. soc. Psychol., 1937, 8, 389–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Survey of objective studies of psychoanalytic concepts. Soc. Sci. Res. Counc., 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Effects of frustration and anxiety on fantasy aggression. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 1951, 21, 498–505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. Social behavior and personality development. In T. Parsons & E. A. Shils (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. A theoretical framework for personality and social behavior. Amer. Psychologist, 1951, 6, 476–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R., & Wise, G. W. The relation of cup feeding in infancy to thumb-sucking and the oral drive. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 1950, 20, 123–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

7. Some Problems in the Theory of Learning

  • Estes, W. K., Koch, S., MacCorquodale, K., Meehl, P. E., Mueller, C. G., Jr., Schoenfeld, W. N., & Verplanck, W. S. Modern learning theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. Theories of learning. (2nd ed.) New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kentucky Symposium. Learning theory, personality theory, and clinical research. New York: Wiley, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. W. Learning. In W. S. Monroe (Ed.), Encyclopedia of educational research. (Rev. ed.) New York: Macmillan, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, E. B. Learning. In H. Helson (Ed.), Theoretical foundations of psychology. New York: Van Nostrand, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Theoretical interpretations of learning. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. Behavior theory and conditioning (Silliman Lectures). New Haven: Yale University Press, 1956.

    Google Scholar 

8. The Current Status of Learning Theory

  • Bower, G. H. (Ed.) Psychology of Learning and Motivation. London: Academic, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, A. J. Classical conditioning in animals. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 1978, 29, 587–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (Ed.) Handbook of learning and cognitive processes. New York: Earlbaum, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honig, W. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. (Eds.) Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. Foundations of conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, R. C. Animal learning: An overview. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973, pp. 461–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prokasy, W. F. (Ed.) Classical conditioning: A Symposium. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, B. F. Theories of learning: A consumer report. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973, pp. 451–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. About behaviorism. New York: Knopf, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolman, B. B. (Ed.) International encyclopedia of psychiatry, psychology, psychoanalysis and neurology. New York: Aesculapius, 1977, 12 vols.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1981 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wolman, B.B. (1981). Neo-Behaviorism and Learning Theory. In: Contemporary Theories and Systems in Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3821-5_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3821-5_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-3823-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4684-3821-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics