Mind, Brain, and Consciousness: The Organization of Competence and Conduct

  • Karl H. Pribram

Abstract

The history of psychology in this century can be charted in terms of the issue that dominated each decade of exploration. Early studies on classical conditioning and Gestalt principles of perception were followed subsequently by two decades of behaviorism. In the 1950s information measurement took the stage to be supplanted in the 1960s by an almost frenetic endeavor to catalogue memory processes, an endeavor which culminated in the new concepts of a cognitive psychology. Currently, the study of consciousness as central to the mind-brain problem has emerged from the explorations of altered and alternative states produced by drugs, meditation, and a variety of other techniques designed to promote psychological growth.

Keywords

Entropy Morphine Retina Peri Macromolecule 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ashby, W. R. Design for a brain. New York: Wiley, 1960.Google Scholar
  2. Bohm, D. Quantum theory as an indication of a new order in physics. Part A. The development of new orders as shown through the history of physics. Foundations of Physics, 1971, 1(4), 359–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bohm, D. Quantum theory as an indication of a new order in physics. Part B. Implicate and explicate order in physical law. Foundations of Physics, 1973, 3(2), 139–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bohr, N. Atomic physics and human knowledge. New York: Vintage Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  5. Bracewell, R. The Fourier transform and its applications. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965.Google Scholar
  6. Brentano, F. The distinction between mental and physical phenomena. In R. M. Chisholm (Ed.), Realism and the background of phenomenology. New York: The Free Press, 1960.Google Scholar
  7. Brillouin, L. Science and information theory. New York: Academic, 1962.Google Scholar
  8. Broadbent, D. E. Division of function and integration. Neurosciences study program, III. New York: The M.I.T. Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  9. Campbell, F. W., & Rosson, J. G. Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. Journal of Physiology, 1968, 197, 551–566.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Dimond, S. J., & Beaumont, J. G. Hemisphere function in the human brain. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  11. Eccles, J. C. Facing reality. New York, Heidelberg, Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1970.Google Scholar
  12. Freeman, W. J. Correlation of electrical activity of prepyriform cortex and behavior in cat. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1960, 23: 111–131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Freeman, W. Mass action in the nervous system. New York: Academic, 1975.Google Scholar
  14. Freud, S. Project for a scientific psychology. Appendix in The origins of psychoanalysis, letters to Wilhelm Fliess, draft and notes 1887–1902. New York: Basic Books, 1954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gabor, D. A new microscopic principle, Nature, 1948, 161, 777–778.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Heisenberg, W. Physics and philosophy. London: Allen and Unwin, 1959.Google Scholar
  17. Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 1962, 160, 106–154PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Isaacson, R. L., & Pribram, K. H. (Eds.), The hippocampus: Vol. I: Neurophysiology and behavior. New York: Plenum, 1976.Google Scholar
  19. James, W. The principles of psychology. London: Macmillan, 1901.Google Scholar
  20. Leith, E. N. White light holograms. Scientific American, 1976, 235(4), 80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two, or some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, 63(2), 81–97.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Henry Holt, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Milner, B. Interhemispheric difference in the localization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 1971, 27(3), 272–277.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Pitts, W., & Mcculloch, W. S. How we know universals. The perception of auditory and visual forms. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 1947, 9, 127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pribram, K. H. A review of theory in physiological psychology. Reprinted from Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. II, 1960, 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pribram, K. H. Proposal for a structural pragmatism: Some neuropsychological considerations of problems in philosophy. In B. Wolman & E. Nagle (Eds.), Scientific psychology: Principles and approaches. New York: Basic Books, 1965.Google Scholar
  27. Pribram, K. H. Some dimensions of remembering: Toward a neuropsychological model of memory. In J. Gaito (Ed.), Macromolecules and behavior. New York: Academic, 1966.Google Scholar
  28. Pribram, K. H. What makes man human. New York: American Museum of Natural History, 1970 James Arthur Lecture.Google Scholar
  29. Pribram, K. H. Languages of the brain: Experimental paradoxes and principles in neuropsychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1971.Google Scholar
  30. Pribram, K. H. How is it that sensing so much we can do so little? In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences study program, III, Cambridge: The M.I.T. Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  31. Pribram, K. H. Problems concerning the structure of consciousness. In G. Globus, G. Maxwell, & I. Savodnik (Eds.), Science and the mind-brain puzzle. New York: Plenum, 1976.Google Scholar
  32. Pribram, K. H. Self-consciousness and intentionality: A model based on an experimental analysis of the brain mechanisms involved in the Jamesian theory of motivation and emotion. In G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation, advances in research, Vol. I, New York: Plenum, 1976.Google Scholar
  33. Pribram, K. H. Holonomy and structure in the organization of perception. Proceedings of the Conference on Images, Perception and Knowledge, University of Western Ontario, May 1974, 1976.Google Scholar
  34. Pribram, K. H. Peptides and protocritic processes. In L. H. Miller, C. A. Sandman, & A. J. Kastin (Eds.), Neuropeptide inflences on the brain and behavior. New York: Raven, 1977, pp. 213–232.Google Scholar
  35. Pribram, K. H., & Gill, M. M. Freud’s ‘project’ re-assessed. New York: Basic Books, 1976.Google Scholar
  36. Pribram, K. H., & Mcguinness, D. Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review, 1975, 82(2), 116–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pribram, K. H., Nuwer, M., & Baron, R. The holographic hypothesis of memory structure in brain function and perception. In R. C. Atkinson, D. H. Krantz, R. C. Luce, & P. Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1974.Google Scholar
  38. Rall, W. Dendritic neuron theory and dendro-dendritic synapses in a simple cortical system. In F. O. Schmitt (Ed.), The neurosciences: Second study program. New York: The Rockefeller University Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  39. Ryle, G. The Concept of mind. New York: Barnes & Noble, 1949.Google Scholar
  40. Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana, Ill.: University of Illinois Press, 1949.Google Scholar
  41. Shepherd, G. M. The synaptic organization of the brain An introduction. New York: Oxford University Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  42. Sherrington, C. Man on his nature. New York: Macmillan, 1941.Google Scholar
  43. Sidman, M., Stoddard, L. T., & Mohr, J. P. Some additional quantitative observations of immediate memory in a patient with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 1968, 6, 245–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sperry, R. W. Mental phenomena as causal determinants in brain function. In Gordon G. Globus, Grover Maxwell, & Irwin Savodnik (Eds.), Consciousness and the brain. New York: Plenum, 1976.Google Scholar
  45. Spinelli, D. N., Pribram, K. H., & Bridgeman, B. Visual receptive field organization of single units in the visual cortex of monkey. International Journal Neuroscience, 1970, 1, 67–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Penfield, W. Mystery of the mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  47. Von Bekesy, G. Sensory inhibition. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1967.Google Scholar
  48. Von Uexkull, J. In R. M. Chisholm (Ed.), Realism and the background of phenomenology. New York: The Free Press, 1960.Google Scholar
  49. Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. Organizational aspects of memory in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 1971, 9, 67–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshall, J. Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 1974, 97(4), 709–728.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Whitehead, A. N. Modes of thought. New York: Capricorn Books, 1958.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1980

Authors and Affiliations

  • Karl H. Pribram
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyStanford UniversityStanfordUSA

Personalised recommendations