Advertisement

The Vocal and Behavioral Reactions of the Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, to Playback of its Sounds

  • David W. Morgan

Abstract

The playback of natural sounds has been used as an experimental technique with diverse groups of animals and with varied degrees of success to elucidate the biological significance of the sounds. Additionally, the playback of pure tones has been used to investigate which parameters of an acoustic signal are directly concerned with communication. Such parameters have included frequency limits of hearing, effectiveness of auditory masking, direction-finding and frequency discrimination capabilities, and intensity limits of frequency detection (audiograms).

Keywords

Killer Whale Bottlenose Dolphin Behavioral Reaction Playback Experiment Captive Animal 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Caldwell, M. C., and Caldwell, D. K., 1964, Experimental studies on factors involved in care-giving behavior in three species of the cetacean family Delphinidae, Bull. South. Calif. Acad. Sci. 63:1–20.Google Scholar
  2. Caldwell, M. C., and Caldwell, D. K., 1967, Intraspecific transfer of information via the pulsed sound in captive Odontocete cetaceans, in: Animal Sonar Systems: Biology and Bionics (R. G. Busnel, ed.), pp. 879–936, Vol. II, N.A.T.O. Advanced Study Institute, Jouy-en-Josas, France.Google Scholar
  3. Caldwell, M. C., Haugen, R. M., and Caldwell, D. K., 1962, High-energy sound associated with fright in the dolphin, Science 138:907–908.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K., and Siebenaler, J. B., 1965, Observations on captive and wild Atlantic bottlenosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, in the northeastern Gulf of Mexico, Los Angeles County Mus. Contrib. Sci. 91:1–10.Google Scholar
  5. Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K., and Hall, N. R., 1972a, Ability of an Atlantic bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) to discriminate between, and potentially identify to individual, the whistles of another species, the common dolphin (Delphinus delphis), Marine land Res. Lab. Tech. Rep. 9:1–7.Google Scholar
  6. Caldwell, M. C., Hall, N. R., and Caldwell, D. K., 1972b, Ability of an Atlantic bottlenosed dolphin to discriminate between, and respond differentially to, whistles of eight conspecifics, Marineland Res. Lab. Tech. Rep. 10:1–5.Google Scholar
  7. Cummings, W. C., and Thompson, P. O., 1971, Gray whales, Eschrichtius robustus, avoid the underwater sounds of killer whales, Orcinus orca, U.S. Fish. Bull. 69:525–530.Google Scholar
  8. Davies, D. H., 1962, Note on the use of killer whale sounds as a shark repellent, S. Afr. Assoc. Mar. Biol. Res. Bull. 3:32–33.Google Scholar
  9. Dreher, J. J., 1966, Cetacean communication: Small-group experiment, in: Whales, Dolphins, and Porpoises (K. S. Norris, ed.), pp. 597–602, University of California Press, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  10. Dudok van Heel, W. H., 1959, Audio-direction finding in the porpoise (Phocoena phocoena), Nature, London 183:1063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Evans, W. E., and Dreher, J. J., 1962, Observations on scouting behavior and associated sound production on the Pacific bottlenose porpoise (Tursiops gilli, Dall), Bull. South. Calif. Acad. Sci. 61:217–226.Google Scholar
  12. Fish, J. F., and Vania, J. S., 1971, Killer whale, Orcinus orca, sounds repel white whales, Delphinapterus leucas, U.S. Fish. Bull. 69:531–535.Google Scholar
  13. Fish, M. P., and Mowbray, W. H., 1962, Production of underwater sound by the white whale or beluga, Delphinapterus leucas (Pallas), J. Mar. Res. 20:149–162.Google Scholar
  14. Hall, J. D., and Johnson, C. S., 1972, Auditory thresholds of a killer whale, Orcinus orca Linnaceus, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 51:515–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jacobs, D. W., 1972, Auditory frequency discrimination in the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus Montague: A preliminary report, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 52:696–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jacobs, D. W., and Hall, J. D., 1972, Auditory thresholds of a fresh water dolphin, Inia geoffrensis Blainville, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 51:530–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Johnson, C. S., 1967, Sound detection thresholds in marine mammals, in: Marine Bio-Acoustics (W. N. Tavolga, ed.), Vol. 2, pp. 247–260, Pergamon Press, New York.Google Scholar
  18. Johnson, C. S., 1968, Masked tonal thresholds in the bottlenosed porpoise, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 44:965–967.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kellogg, W. N., 1953, Ultrasonic hearing in the porpoise, Tursiops truncatus, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 46:446–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kellogg, W. N., and Kohler, R., 1952, Reactions of the porpoise to ultrasonic frequencies, Science 116:250–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kleinenberg, S. E., Yablokov, A. V., Bel’kovich, B. M., and Tarasevich, M. N., 1964, Beluga (Delphinapterus leucas), Investigation of the Species, translated by Israel Program for Scientific Translations, 376 pp.Google Scholar
  22. Lang, T. G., and Smith, H. A. P., 1965, Communication between dolphins in separate tanks by way of an electronic acoustic link, Science 150:1839–1844.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lemon, R. E., 1967, The response of cardinals to songs of different dialects, Anim. Behav. 15:538–545.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lilly, J. C., 1963, Distress calls of the bottlenose dolphin: Stimuli and evoked behavioral response, Science 139:116–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lilly, J. C., and Miller, A. M., 1961, Vocal exchanges between dolphins, Science 134:1873–1876.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Morgan, D. W., 1970, The reactions of belugas to natural sound playbacks, Proc. 7th Ann. Conf. Biol. Sonar and Diving Mammals, pp. 61–66, Stanford Research Institute, Menlo Park, California.Google Scholar
  27. Morgan, D. W., 1973, The vocal and behavior reactions of the white whale, or beluga, (Delphinapterus leucas) to underwater playback of natural and synthetic beluga sounds, PhD thesis, University of Rhode Island.Google Scholar
  28. Schevill, W. E., and Lawrence, B., 1949, Underwater listening to the white porpoise (Delphinapterus leucas), Science 109:143–144.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Schevill, W. E., and Lawrence, B., 1950, A phonograph record of the underwater calls of Delphinapterus leucas, Woods Hole Oceanogr. Inst. Ref. No. 50–1.Google Scholar
  30. Schevill, W. E., and Lawrence, B., 1953, Auditory response of a bottlenosed porpoise, Tursiops truncatus, to frequencies above 100 kc, J. Exp. Zool. 124:147–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Siegel, S., 1956, Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, McGraw-Hill, New York. 312 pp.Google Scholar
  32. Watkins, W. A., and Schevill, W. E., 1968, Underwater playback of their own sounds to Leptonychotes (Weddell seals), J. Mammal. 49:287–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1979

Authors and Affiliations

  • David W. Morgan
    • 1
  1. 1.Aquatic Behavior Laboratory, Department of BiologyUniversity of Notre DameNotre DameUSA

Personalised recommendations