Advertisement

Discrete trial analysis of drug action

  • George A. Heise
Part of the FASEB Monographs book series (FASEBM, volume 4)

Abstract

Discrete trial procedures permit exact control or description of the time of occurrence of stimuli, the probability of response occurrence, and the patterning of responses. They also make possible the experimental manipulation of the composition of the stimuli controlling behavior. The use of discrete trial procedures is illustrated here in an examination of the effects of scopolamine, a representative cholinergic blocker, on several aspects of behavior: Memory. Response alternation experiments, in which the spacing of discrete trials varies within the experimental session, show that, whereas accuracy of responding is consistently poorer under drug, the decline of accuracy with time since last trial is similar for drugged and nondrugged animals. Thus the drug does not affect memory “storage.” Inhibition. Experiments in which discrete trials are presented in pairs, such that the correct response on Trial 2 of the pair is contingent upon Trial 1 events, show how the “disinhibiting” effect of scopolamine (as indicated by enhanced responding on “no go” trials) is augmented by increasing the time gap between Trial 1 and Trial 2, or by minimizing controlling stimuli on Trial 1. Discrimination. A variety of experiments suggest that scopolamine decreases the “detectability” of stimuli. Detectability effects, along with disinhibition observed under certain specific conditions, constitute the principal behavioral actions of scopolamine observed with discrete trial procedures. — Heise, G. A. Discrete trial analysis of drug action. Federation Proc. 34: 1898–1903, 1975.

Keywords

Intertrial Interval Preceding Trial Discrete Trial Variable Interval Schedule Discrete Trial Procedure 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Brown, K., and D. M. Warburton. Attenuation of stimulus sensitivity by scopolamine. Psychonomic Sci. 22: 297, 1971.Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Carlton, P. L. Cholinergic mechanisms in the control of behavior by the brain. Psychol. Rev. 70: 19, 1963.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    D’amato, M. R. Delayed matching and short-term memory in monkeys. In: The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, Vol. 7, edited by G. H. Bower. New York: Academic, 1973, p. 227.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Dews, P. B. Studies on Behavior. I. Differential Sensitivity to Pentobarbital of Pecking Performance in Pigeons Depending on the Schedule of Reward. J. Pharmac. Exp. Ther. 113: 393, 1955.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Glick, S. D., and M. E. Jarvik. Differential effects of amphetamine and scopolamine on matching performance of monkeys with lateral frontal lesions. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 73: 307, 1970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Hearst, E. Effects of scopolamine on discriminated responding in the rat. J. Pharmac. Exp. Ther. 126: 349, 1959.Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Heise, G. A., and R. Conner. Scopolamine effects on time dependent processes (“memory storage”) in the rat measured in a variable interval spatial alternation task. Presented at Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, May 10–12, 1973, Chicago, Illinois.Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Heise, G. A., N. Laughlin and E. Keller. A behavioral and pharmacological analysis of reinforcement withdrawal. Psychopharmacologia 16: 345, 1970.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Innes, I. R., and M. Nickerson. Drugs inhibiting the action of acetylcholine on structures innervated by postganglionic parasympathetic nerves (antimuscarinic or atropinic drugs). In: The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics, edited by L. S. Goodman and A. Gilman. New York: Macmillan, 1965, 3rd ed, p. 521.Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Kelleher, R. T., and W. H. Morse. Determinants of the specificity of behavioral effects of drugs. Ergeb. Physiol. Biol. Chem. Exp. Pharmakol. 60: 1, 1968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Krank, M., G. Christoph and S. Leinwand. Proactive influences on spatial alternation in the rat. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, Illinois, May 2–4, 1974.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Laties, V. G., and B. Weiss. Influence of drugs on behavior controlled by internal and external stimuli. J. Pharmac. Exp. Ther. 152: 388, 1966.Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Warburton, D. M. The cholinergic control of internal inhibition. In: Inhibition and Learning, edited by R. Boakes and M. S. Halliday. London: Academic, 1972, p. 431.Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Warburton, D. M., and K. Brown. Scopolamine-induced attenuation of stimulus sensitivity. Nature 230: 126, 1971.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Weiskrantz, L. Memory. In: Analysis of Behavioral Change, edited by L. Weiskrantz. New York: Harper and Row, 1968, p. 158.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1975

Authors and Affiliations

  • George A. Heise
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIndiana UniversityBloomingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations