Skip to main content

Effects of Exposure to Spatially Distorted Stimuli

  • Chapter
  • 361 Accesses

Part of the book series: Perception and Perceptual Development ((PPD,volume 1))

Abstract

The successful execution of most pieces of behavior requires that the animal correctly perceive the positions and movements of its own body parts and the positions and movements of external objects. Many spatial skills are so vital that they are innately determined, or require very little experience for their development. For instance, the wildebeest is able to run after its mother within hours of being born, and most mammals avoid the edges of cliffs that they can see, even when they encounter them for the first time. Nevertheless, whether particular spatial perceptual mechanisms are innate or not, they possess a degree of flexibility; i.e., they may be temporarily, or permanently, modified by experience. Such flexibility is required to correct for the changes that occur when an animal grows, is injured, or encounters unusual types of stimulation. It is also required to correct for the tendency of any complex system to drift from its peak performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Annis, R. C., and Frost, B. Human visual ecology and orientation anisotropics in acuity. Science, 1973, 182, 729–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Appelle, S. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 78, 266–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E., and Witkin, H. A. Studies in space orientation. II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 455–477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baily, J. S. Arm-body adaptation with passive arm movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 1972, 72, 39–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, H. B. Visual experience and cortical development. Nature, 1975, 258, 199–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, C., and Cooper, G. Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature, 1970, 228, 477–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, T. G. R. Development in infancy. San Francisco: Freeman, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, F. W., and Maffei, L. The tilt after-effect: A fresh look. Vision Research, 1971, 77, 833–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canon, L. K. Intermodality inconsistency of input and directed attention as determinants of the nature of adaptation. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1970, 84, 141–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, J. K. Isolation of the muscular component in a proprioceptive spatial aftereffect. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1911, 90, 297–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. Visual feature-analyzers and aftereffects of tilt and curvature. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 114–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., and Cooper, C. M. The retinal reference of the tilt aftereffect. Perception and Psychophysics, 1972, 11, 321–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L. A. Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation. Perception and Psychophysics, 1976, 79, 296–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S. Adaptation to prismatic displacement as a function of the amount of available information. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 4, 407–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craske, B. Adaptation to prisms: Change in internally registered eye-position. British Journal of Psychology, 1967, 55, 329–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewar, R. Adaptation to displaced vision: Variations on the prismatic-shaping technique. Perception and Psychophysics, 1971, 9, 155–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ebenholtz, S. M., and Wolfson, D. M. Perceptual effects of sustained convergence. Perception and Psychophysics, 1975, 77, 485–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, W., and Morgan-Paap, C. A. The effect of level of depth processing and degree of informational discrepancy on adaptation to uniocular image magnification. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1974, 102, 585–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorentini, A., Ghez, C., and Maffei, L. Physiological correlates of adaptation to a rotated visual field. Journal of Physiology, 1972, 227, 313–322.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, G. H. Resolution of spatial conflict. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 1962, 46, 3A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, G. M., and Robinson, D. A. Adaptation of the human vestibuloocular reflex to magnifying lenses. Brain Research, 1975, 92, 331–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. L. Eye and brain. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halper-Smith, C. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Stanford University, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, C. R. Intermanual transfer of adaptation to prisms. American Journal of Psychology, 1964, 77, 457–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardt, M. E., Held, R., and Steinbach, M. J. Adaptation to displaced vision: A change in the central control of sensorimotor coordination. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1971, 89, 229–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. S. Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. Psychological Review, 1965, 72, 419–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. C. Visual adaptation to an altered correlation between eye movement and head movement. Science, 1968, 160, 429–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. C., Pick, H. L., and Roser, E. Adaptation to chromatic aberration by the human visual system. Science, 1963, 141, 167–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. C., Pick, H. L., and Ikeda, K. Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 2, 215–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hein, A., and Held, R. A neural model for labile sensorimotor coordinations. In Biological prototypes and synthetic systems. Vol. I. New York: Plenum Press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hein, A., Gower, E. C., and Diamond, R. H. Exposure requirements for developing the triggered component of the visual-placing response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970a, 73, 188–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hein, A., Held, R., and Gower, E. C. Development and segmentation of visually controlled movement by selective exposure during rearing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970b, 73, 181–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R. Exposure history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1961, 132, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., and Hein, A. Adaptation of disarranged hand-eye coordination contingent upon re-afferent stimulation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1958, 8, 87–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., and Hein, A. Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behaviour. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 872–876.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P. Displacing the optical array. In S. J. Freedman (Ed.), The neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior. Homewood 111.: Dorsey, 1968, pp. 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P. Perceptual learning and adaptation. British Medical Bulletin, 1971, 27, 248–252.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P. Proposals for the study of adaptation to anomalous schemata. Perception, 1975, 3, 497–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P., and Anstis, T. Muscular and joint-receptor components in postural persistence. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1914, 103, 167–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P., and Templeton, W. B. The effect of fixation on the judgment of relative depth. Quarterly Journal of experimental Psychology, 1964, 16, 193–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P., and Templeton, W. B. Human spatial orientation. London: Wiley, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P., Craske, B., and Templeton, W. B. Visuo-motor adaptation to discordant ex-afferent stimulation. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 189–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, I. P., Anstis, T., and Lucia, H. C. The relative lability of mobile and stationary components in a visual-motor adaptation task. Quarterly Journal of experimental Psychology, 1974, 26, 293–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 1962, 160, 106–154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, C. V. Visual factors in auditory localization. Quarterly Journal of experimental Psychology, 1953, 5, 52–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A., Cook, E., Olson, M. E., and Epstein, W. Allocation of attention and the locus of adaptation to displaced vision. Journal of experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975, 104, 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, J. M. Prismatic displacement and the remembered location of targets. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 218–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, I. Experiments with prolonged optical distortions. Acta Psychologica, 1955, 11, 176–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A., and Perkins, D. N. Orientation of letters and errors in their recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 265–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornhuber, H. H. (Ed.) Handbook of sensory physiology. Vol. VI, Part 1: Vestibular system. New York: Springer, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kottenhoff, H. Situational and personal influences on space perception with experimental spectacles. 1. Prolonged experiments with inverting glasses. Acta Psychologica, 1957, 13, 79–97, 370–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kottenhoff, H. Was ist richtiges Sehen mit Umkehrbrillen und in welchem Sinne stellt sich das Sehen uml Meisenheim am Glan, Germany: Anton Hain, 1961.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravitz, J. H., and Wallach, H. Adaptation to displaced vision contingent upon vibrating stimulation. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 6, 465–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J. R. Adaptation to displaced vision: Role of proprioception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1974, 38, 1251–1256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leehey, S. C., Moskowitz-Cook, A., Brill, S., and Held, R. Orientational anistropy in infant vision. Science, 1975, 190, 900–901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal, A. G., and Hirsch, H. V. Cortical effect of early selective exposure to diagonal lines. Science, 1975, 190, 902–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R. Cat’s brains are controversial. New Scientist, 1975, 20, 457–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. M. Voluntary eye movements as questions. In J. Dichgans and E. Bizzi (Eds.), Cerebral control of eye movements and motion perception. Basel: Karger, 1972, pp. 369–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, W. D., Farias, M. C. Williams, W. J., and Bement, S. L. Static and dynamic responses of slowly adapting joint receptors. Brain Research, 1974, 70, 221–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melamed, L. E., Haley, M., and Gildow, J. W. Effect of external target presence on visual adaptation with active and passive movement. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1973, 98, 125–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metzler, J., and Shepard, R. N. Transformational studies of the internal representation of three- dimensional objects. In R. Solso (Ed.), Theories of cognitive psychology. Potomac, Md.: Erl- baum, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. A. Interaction of vision and touch in conflict and nonconflict form perception tasks. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1972, 96, 114–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, D. E., Freeman, R. D., Millodot, M., and Haegerstrom, G. Meridional amblyopia: Evidence for modification of the human visual system by early experience. Visual Research, 1973, 13, 535–558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moulden, B. Adaptation to displaced vision: Reafference is a special case of the cue-discrepancy hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of experimental Psychology, 1971, 25, 113–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V. B., Poggio, G. F., and Werner, G. The relation of thalamic cell response to peripheral stimuli varied over an intensive continuum. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1963, 26, 807–834.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. I. Volition: A new experimental approach. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 4, 225–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogle, K. N. Binocular vision. New York: Hafner, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Over, R. An experimentally induced conflict between vision and proprioception. British Journal of Psychology, 1966, 57, 335–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pick, H. L., and Hay, J. C. A passive test of the Held reafference hypothesis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, 1070–1072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pick, H. L., Warren, D. H., and Hay, J. C. Sensory conflicts in judgments of spatial direction. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 6, 203–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pick, H. L., Jr., Warren, D. H., Mclntyre, C., and Appel, L. Transfer and the organization of perceptual-motor space. Psycholgische Forschung, 1972, 35, 163–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radeau, M. The locus of adaptation to auditory-visual conflict. Perception, 1973, 2, 327–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radeau, M. Differences in visual and auditory adaptation to prismatic displacement. Année Psychologique, 1974, 74, 23–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radeau, M., and Bertelson, P. Adaptation à un déplacement prismatique sur la base de stimulations exafférentes en conflit. Psychologica Belgica, 1969, 9, 133–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radeau, M., and Bertelson, P. The effects of a textured visual field on modality dominance in a ventriloquism situation. Perception and Psychophysics, 1976, 20, 227–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. The nature of perceptual adaptation. New York: Basic Books, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. Orientation and form. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., and Victor, J. Vision and touch: An experimentally created conflict between the two senses. Science, 1964, 143, 594–596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skowbo, D., Timney, B. N., Gentry, T. A., and Morant, R. B. McCollough effects: Experimental findings and theoretical accounts. Psychological Bulletin, 1975, 82, 497–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. U., and Smith, W. M. Perception and motion. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stratton, G. M. Vision without inversion of the retinal image. Psychological Review, 1897, 4, 341–363, 363–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. G. The behavioral basis of perception. New York: Yale University Press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, W. B., Howard, I. P., and Lowman, A. E. Passively generated adaptation to prismatic distortion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 22, 140–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, W. B., Howard, I. P., and Wilkinson, D. A. Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation. Perception and Psychophysics, 1974, 75, 249–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurlow, W. R., and Jack, C. E. Certain determinants of the ventriloquism effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1973, 36, 1171–1184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Timney, B. N., and Muir, D. W. Orientation anisotrophy: Incidence and magnitude in Caucasian and Chinese subjects. Science, 1976, 193, 699–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlarik, J. J. Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1973, 98, 223–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlarik, J. J., and Canon, L. K. Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. Journal of experimental Psychology,, 1971, 91, 233–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Laer, E. S., Swartz, A., and Van Laer, J. Adaptation to prismatically displaced vision as a function of degree of displacement and amount of feedback. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 30, 723–728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Hoist, E. Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 1954, 2, 89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. Preexposure pointing frequency effects on adaptation to prismatic viewing. Perception and Psychophysics, 1974, 15, 26–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., and Flaherty, E. W. Covariance as a principle in perceptual adaptation. Psychologia, 1974, 17, 159–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., and Frey, K. J. Adaptation in distance perception based on oculomotor cues. Perception and Psychophysics, 1972, 77, 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., Stanton, L., and Becker, D. The compensation for movement-produced changes of object orientation. Perception and Psychophysics, 1974, 75, 339–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R. B. Adaptation to prism-displaced vision: The importance of target-pointing. Perception and Psychophysics, 1969, 5, 305–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R. B. The effect of experienced limb identity upon adaptation to simulated displacement of the visual field. Perception and Psychophysics, 1972, 72, 453–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R. B., and Abel, M. R. The generality of the target-pointing effect in prism adaptation. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 20, 226–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, D. A. The visual-motor control loops: A linear system? Journal of experimental Psychology, 1971, 59, 250–257. Willott, J. F. Perceptual judgments with discrepant information from audition and proprioception.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perception and Psychophysics, 1973, 14, 577–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H. A., Wapner, S., and Leventhal, T. Sound localization with conflicting visual and auditory cues. Journal of experimental Psychology, 1952, 43, 58–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1978 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Howard, I.P. (1978). Effects of Exposure to Spatially Distorted Stimuli. In: Walk, R.D., Pick, H.L. (eds) Perception and Experience. Perception and Perceptual Development, vol 1. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-2619-9_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-2619-9_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-2621-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4684-2619-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics