Advertisement

Auditory Environment and Vocal Development in Birds

  • Masakazu Konishi
Part of the Perception and Perceptual Development book series (PPD, volume 1)

Abstract

Bird voice, a seemingly esoteric topic for scientific inquiry, is known to exhibit some of the most interesting phenomena of behavioral development such as the effects of sensory exposure, deprivation, and isolation, inborn perceptual preference, and critical impressionable period. Sexual imprinting in birds shares some of these attributes (Immelman, 1972a,b). The development of response properties in the cat visual cortex is another example (Wiesel and Hubel, 1965; Barlow, 1975; see also Mitchell, this volume). The perception of speech sounds in human infants seems to show some similarities to song development in birds (Eimas, 1975; see also Strange and Jenkins, this volume). The study of developmental plasticity in avian vocalizations has been greatly facilitated both by the objective means of describing voice and by the ease with which auditory environment can be manipulated. The results suggest that bird song can be a useful model to obtain deeper insights into the general principles of behavioral development. The main aim of this chapter is to discuss the ontogeny of bird voice as it relates to these principles rather than to present a descriptive review of the field, which is already available (Konishi and Nottebohm, 1969; Marler and Mundinger, 1971).

Keywords

Zebra Finch Auditory Feedback Young Bird Auditory Environment Song Sparrow 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

11. References

  1. Baptista, L. F. Song dialects and demes in sedentary populations of the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophyrys nuttali). University of California Publications in Zoology, 1975, 105, 1–52.Google Scholar
  2. Barlow, H. B. Visual experience and cortical development. Nature, 1975, 258, 199–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bäumer, E. Lebensart des Haushuhns, III—Über seine Laute und allgemeine Ergänzungen. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1962, 19, 394–416.Google Scholar
  4. Eimas, P. D. Speech perception in early infancy. In L. B. Cohen and P. Salapatek (Eds.), Perception. New York: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  5. Immelmann, K. Prägungserscheinungen in der Gesangsentwicklung junger Zebrafinken. Naturwis-senschaften, 1965, 52, 169–170.Google Scholar
  6. Immelmann, K. Zur ontogenetischen Gesangsentwicklung bei Prachtfinken. Verhandlungen der deutschen Zoologischen Gesellschaft, Göttingen, 1967, 1966, 320–332.Google Scholar
  7. Immelmann, K. Song development in the zebra finch and other estrildid finches. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Bird vocalizations. London: Cambridge University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  8. Immelmann, K. The influence of early experience upon the development of social behaviour in estrildine finches. Proceedings of the XVth International Ornithological Congress, 1972a, pp. 316–338.Google Scholar
  9. Immelmann, K. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 1912b, 4, 147–174.Google Scholar
  10. Konishi, M. The role of auditory feedback in the vocal behavior of the domestic fowl. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1963, 20, 349–367.Google Scholar
  11. Konishi, M. Effects of deafening on song development in two species of juncos. Condor, 1964, 66, 85–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Konishi, M. Effects of deafening on song development in American robins and black-headed grosbeaks. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1965a, 22, 584–599.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Konishi, M. The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1965b, 22, 770–783.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Konishi, M., and Nottebohm, F. Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocalizations. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Bird vocalizations. London: Cambridge University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  15. Kroodsma, D. E. Re-evaluation of song development in the song sparrow. Animal Behaviour, 1977, 25, 390–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lanyon, W. E. The comparative biology of the meadowlarks (Sturnella) in Wisconsin. Publication of the Nuttall Ornithological Club, 1957, 7, 1–67.Google Scholar
  17. Lemon, R. E. Geographical variation in the song of cardinals. Canadian Journal of Zoology,. 1966, 44, 413–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lemon, R. E., and Scott, D. M. On the development of song in young cardinals. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 1966, 44, 191–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Marler, P. Species distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour, 1957, 77, 13–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Marler, P. Bird songs and mate selection. In W. E. Lanyon and W. N. Tavolga (Eds.), Animal sounds and communication. Washington, D.C.: American Institute of Biological Sciences, 1960, pp. 348–367.Google Scholar
  21. Marler, P. A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Monograph, 1970, 77(2), Part 2, 1–25.Google Scholar
  22. Marler, P., and Mundinger, P. Vocal learning in birds. In H. Moltz (Ed.), The ontogeny of vertebrate behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  23. Marler, P., and Tamura, M. Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal behavior in sparrows. Science, 1964, 746, 1483–1486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Marler, P., and Tamura, M. Song dialects in three populations of white-crowned sparrows. Condor, 1962, 64, 363–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Marler, P., Kreith, M., and Tamura, M. Song development in hand-raised Oregon juncos. Auk, 1962, 79, 12–30.Google Scholar
  26. Mulligan, J. A. Singing behavior and its development in the song sparrow Melospiza melodia. University of California Publications in Zoology, 1966, 81, 1–73.Google Scholar
  27. Nottebohm, F. Auditory experience and song development in the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Ibis, 1968, 770, 549–568.Google Scholar
  28. Nottebohm, F. The critical period for song learning in birds. Ibis, 1969, 3, 386–387.Google Scholar
  29. Nottebohm, F., and Nottebohm, M. E. Vocalizations and breeding behaviour of surgically deafened ring doves (Streptopelia risonia). Animal Behaviour, 1971, 19, 313–327.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Nottebohm, F., Stokes, T. M., and Leonard, C. M. Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 1976, 165, 457–486.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. Weitere Beiträge zur Sozial -und Individuel-Psychologie des Haushuhns. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 1923, 92, 60–87.Google Scholar
  32. Schleidt, W. M. Über die Spontaneität von Erbkoordinationen. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1964, 27, 235–256.Google Scholar
  33. Thielcke, G. Geographie variation in bird vocalizations. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Bird vocalizations. London: Cambridge University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  34. Thorpe, W. H. The learning of song patterns by birds, with especial reference to the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ibis, 1958ö, 100, 535–570.Google Scholar
  35. Thorpe, W. H. Further studies on the process of song learning in the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs gengleri). Nature, 19586, 182, 554–557.Google Scholar
  36. Thorpe, W. H. Bird-song. London: Cambridge University Press, 1961.Google Scholar
  37. von Uexktill, J. Umwelt und Innenleben der Tiere. Berlin, 1909.Google Scholar
  38. Wiesel, T. N., and Hubel, D. H. Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1965, 28, 1029–1040.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1978

Authors and Affiliations

  • Masakazu Konishi
    • 1
  1. 1.Division of BiologyCalifornia Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUSA

Personalised recommendations