Skip to main content

Stimuli, the Perceiver, and Perception

  • Chapter
Perception and Experience

Part of the book series: Perception and Perceptual Development ((PPD,volume 1))

Abstract

Virtually every boy who has grown up in a cold climate has had the experience of warming his hands after making snowballs by holding them in water from the cold water tap. Nearly everyone who has been ill with a high fever has noted how cool the hand of the examining physician or nurse feels. And certainly everybody has observed that a flashlight which shines brightly at night will appear dim during daylight, if indeed it is seen at all. These experiences are examples of successive thermal contrast and simultaneous brightness contrast, two of the phenomena with which this chapter is concerned. They illustrate the very important fact that the appearance of a stimulus (the water, the hand, the light) depends not only on its physical intensity but also on the intensity of its immediately surrounding area as well as on that of stimuli immediately previously experienced. As the cold hand approaches the temperature of the tap water, the tap water will cease to feel warm. When it equals that of the tap water no temperature sensation will be experienced at all. And if the hand is further warmed by being placed in water from the hot water tap, water from the cold water tap will then feel cold. These changes in experience illustrate two important psychological principles, namely that perception is relative and depends on some psychological referent or base line and that these psychological base lines change with experience over time.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adamson, R., and Everett, K. Response modification by “irrelevant” stimulus attributes. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 74, 81, 83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, B. F. Cognitive psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avant, L. L. Contrast and assimilation effects in judgments of line configurations containing the

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller-Lyer figure. Perception and Psychophysics 197la, 10 437–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avant, L. L., and Helson, H. Theories of perception. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973, pp. 419–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avant, L. L., and Kent, M. Anchoring lines and the Mueller-Lyer illusion. U.S. Army Human Engineering Laboratories Technical Note No. 6–70, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behar, I., and Bevan, W. The perceived duration of auditory and visual intervals: Cross-modal comparison and interaction American Journal of Psychology, 1961, 74, 17–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. A., and Bevan, W. Influence of anchors upon the operation of certain gestalt organizing principles. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 670–678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berkeley, G. Three dialogues between Hylas and Philonous. London: 1713. Reprint ed. Chicago: Open Court Publishing Company, 1927.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W. An adaptation-level interpretation of reinforcement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Monograph Supplement 3–23, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Adamson, R. Reinforcers and reinforcement: Their relation to maze performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 59, 226–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Adamson, R. E. Internal referents and the concept of reinforcement. In N. F. Washburne (Ed.), Decisions, values, and groups. Vol. 2. New York: Pergamon Press, 1962, pp. 453–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Darby, C. L. Patterns of experience and the constancy of an indifference point for perceived weight. American Journal of Psychology, 1955, 68, 575–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Pritchard, J. F. Effect of “subliminal” tones upon the judgment of loudness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 23 - 29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Pritchard, J. F. The anchor effect and the problem of relevance in the judgment of shape. Journal of General Psychology, 1963b, 69, 147–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., and Saugstad, P. Experience, discrimination, and generalization efficiency, British Journal of Psychology, 1955, 46, 13–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beven, W., Barker, H., and Pritchard, J. F. The Newhall scaling method, psychophysical bowing, and adaptation level. Journal of General Psychology, 1963a, 69, 95–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., Maier, R. A., and Helson, H. The influence of context upon the estimation of number. American Journal of Psychology, 1963b, 78, 464–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., Bell, R. A., and Taylor, C. Changes in response latency following shifts in the pitch of a signal. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 72, 864–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan, W., Avant, L. L., and Lankford, H. G. Influence of interpolated periods of activity and inactivity upon the vigilance decrement. Journal of Applied Psychology 1967, 51 352– 356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Black, R. W., and Bevan, W. The effect of subliminal shock upon the judged intensity of weak shock American Journal of Psychology, 1960, 73, 262–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, R., Adamson, R., and Bevan, W. Runway behavior as a function of apparent intensity of shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1961, 54, 270–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boardman, W. K., and Goldstone, S. Effects of subliminal anchors upon judgments of size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1962, 14, 475–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. Decision and stress. London: Academic Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. R. Stimulus-similarity and the anchoring of subjective scales. American Journal of Psychology, 1953, 66, 199–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. R., and Reich, C. M. Individual differences and adaptation-level theory. In M. H. Appley (Ed.)Adaptation-level theory. New York: Academic Press, 1971, pp. 215–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. C. Human experimental psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., and Krai, T. P. Transposition away from a rewarded stimulus card to a nonrewarded one as a function of a shift in background. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 57, 592–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capehart, J., Tempone, V. J., and Hébert, J. A theory of stimulus equivalence. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 405–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Lollo, V. Contrast effects in judgment of lifted weights. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 383–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinnerstein, D. Intermanual effects of anchors on zones of maximal sensitivity in weight discrimination. American Journal of Psychology, 1965, 78, 66–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinnerstein, D. Adaptation level and structural interaction: Alternative or complementary concepts? In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory. New York: Academic Press, 1971, pp. 81–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, N. F. Subliminal perception: The nature of a controversy. London: McGraw-Hill, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durlach, N. I., and Braida, L. D. Intensity perception. I. Preliminary theory of intensity resolution. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1969, 46, 372–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H., Avant, L. L., and Bevan, W. Does context influence the shape of a perceptual scale? Perception and Psychophysics, 1968, 4, 54 - 56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. D. Adaptation level theory and context effects on sensory judgments: Perception or response? Perception, 1972, 1, 101–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilinsky, A. S. Comment: Adaptation level, contrast, and the moon illusion. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory. New York: Academic Press, 1971, pp. 71–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, S., and Goldfarb, J. L. Adaptation level, personality theory, and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 1964, 61, 176–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, S., Goldfarb, J., Strong, J., and Russell, J. Replication: Effect of subliminal shock upon judged intensity of weak shock. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1962, 14, 222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. A survey of mathematical models in experimental psychology. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of general psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973, pp. 123–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haertzen, C. A., and Hooks, N. T. Contrast effects from simulation of subjective experiences: A possible standard for behavioral modification. British Journal of Addiction, 1971, 66, 225–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardesty, D., and Bevan, W. Response latency as a function of the temporal pattern of stimulation. Psychological Record, 1965, 15, 385 - 392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, O. J., and Campbell, D. T. Judgments of weight as affected by adaptation range, adaptation duration, magnitude of unlabeled anchor, and judgmental language. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 65, 12–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hébert, J. A., Bullock, M., Levitt, L., Woodward, K. G., and McGuirk, F. D. Context and frequency effects in the generalization of a human voluntary response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974, 102, 456–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Fundamental problems in color vision. I. The principle governing changes in hue, saturation, and lightness of non-selective samples in chromatic illumination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1938, 23, 439–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Adaptation-level as a frame of reference for prediction of psychophysical data. American Journal of Psychology, 1947, 60, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Adaptation-level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference. Psychological Review, 1948, 55, 297–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Adaptation level theory. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science. Vol. 1. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1957, pp. 565–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Studies of anomalous contrast and assimilation. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1963, 53, 179–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper and Row, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Current trends and issues in adaptation-level theory. American Psychologist, 1964b, 19, 26–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Adaptation-level theory: 1970 and after. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory. New York: Academic Press, 1971, pp. 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. Some highlights of an intellectual journey. In T. S. Krawiec (Ed.), The psychologists. Vol. 1. New York: Oxford University Press, 1972, pp. 91–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Joy, V. L. Domains of lightness assimilation and contrast. Psychologische Beiträge, 1962, 6, 405–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Kaplan, S. Effects of background reflectance on transposition of lightness discrimination. Unpublished study, 1950. Cited in H. Helson, Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper and Row, 1964, pp. 413–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Kozaki, A. Anchor effects using numerical estimates of simple dot patterns. Perception and Psychophysics 1968 4 163–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Kozaki, T. Effects of duration of series and anchor-stimuli on judgments of perceived size. American Journal of Psychology, 1968b, 81, 291–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Michels, W. C. The effect of adaptation on achromaticity. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1948, 38, 1025–1032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Nash, M. C. Anchor, contrast, and paradoxical distance effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 59, 113–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., and Rohles, F. H., Jr. A quantitative study of reversal of classical lightness-contrast. American Journal of Psychology, 1959, 72, 530–538.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., Bevan, W., and Masters, H. G. A quantitative study of relevance in the formation of adaptation levels. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1966, 22, 743–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinckley, E. D., and Rethlingshafer, D. Value judgments of heights of men by college students. Journal of Psychology, 1951, 31, 257–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurwitz, H. M. B. The effect of illumination conditions on the effectiveness of light-onset as a reinforcer: A test of the Bevan-Adamson reinforcement theory. British Journal of Psychology, 1960, 51, 341–346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, H. An application of Helson’s theory of adaptation level to the problem of transposition. Psychological Review, 1953, 60, 345 - 352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kakizaki, S. Semantic effects in perceptual judgments. Psychologia, 1967, 10, 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. Principles of gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiinnapas, T. M. Influence of frame size on apparent length of a line. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1955, 50, 168–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. An essay concerning human understanding. Book 2, Sect. 21. London: 1690. A later edition, New York: Valentine Seaman, 1824.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDougall, J., and Bevan, W. Influence of pretest shock upon rate of electrical self-stimulation of the brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65, 261–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., and Lowell, E. L. The achievement motive. New York: Appleton Century, 1953.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Michels, W. C., and Helson, H. A reformulation of the Fechner law in terms of adaptation-level applied to rating-scale data. American Journal of Psychology, 1949, 62, 355–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikkonen, V. On the retention of perceptual quantities. Commentationes Humanarum Litterarum, Societas Scientiarum Fennica 1969, 44(3) 92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morikiyo, Y. Time-order error in the successive comparison of tones: An examination of adaptation-level theory. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 1959, 30, 198–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H., Rayman, N., and Bliss, E. L. Preparatory set (expectancy): An experimental demonstration of its central locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1940, 26, 357–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, H. G. Stimulus intensity and reaction time: Evaluation of a decision-theory model. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 84, 383–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parducci, A., and Marshall, L. M. Assimilation vs. contrast in the anchoring of perceptual judgments of weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 426–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parducci, A., and Sandusky, A. J. Limits and the applicability of signal detection theories. Perception and Psychophysics, 1970, 7, 63–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parducci, A., Calfee, R. C., Marshall, L. M., and Davidson, L. P. Context effects in judgment: Adaptation level as a function of mean, midpoint, and median of the stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1960, 60, 65–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Podell, J. E. A comparison of generalization and adaptation-level as theories of connotation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 62, 593–597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, C. C. Time-errors in the method of single stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1933, 16, 798–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, J. F., and Bevan, W. Anchor effectiveness as a function of stimulus variation on an incidental dimension. Journal of General Psychology, 1966, 74, 245–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Restle, F., and Merryman, C. Distance and an illusion of length of line. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 87, 297–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rethlingshafer, D., and Hinckley, E. D. Influence of judges’ characteristics upon adaptation level. American Journal of Psychology, 1963, 76, 116 - 119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., and Brosgole, L. Grouping based on phenomenal proximity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 57, 531–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M., and Thibaut, J. Determinants of standards of judgment. Journal of Personality, 1974, 42, 383–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarris, V. Adaptation-level theory: Two critical experiments on Helson’s weighted-average model. American Journal of Psychology, 1967, 80, 331–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M., Taub, D., and Hovland, C. I. Assimilation and contrast effects of anchoring stimuli on judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 55, 150–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steger, J. A. Reversal of simultaneous lightness-contrast. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 70, 774–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steger, J. A. Visual lightness assimilation and contrast as a function of differential stimulation. American Journal of Psychology, 1969, 82, 56–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steger, J. A., and O’Reilly, E. Simultaneously contrasting anchors. Perception and Psychophysics, 1970, 7, 281–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. S. Adaptation-level vs. the relativity of judgment. American Journal of Psychology, 1958, 71, 633–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. S. To honor Fechner and to repeal his law. Science, 1961, 133, 80–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swets, J. A. Central factors in auditory frequency selectivity. Psychological Bulletin, 1963, 60, 429–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swets, J. A. Comment: Adaptation-level theory and signal-detection theory and their relation to vigilance experiments. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory. New York: Academic Press, 1971, pp. 49–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. R., and Jones, C. G. Stimulus generalization as a function of the frame of reference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 77 - 80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. R., Strub, H., and Dickson, J. F., Jr. Adaptation level and the central tendency effect in stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1974, 103, 466–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, E. D., and Bevan, W. Simultaneous induction of multiple anchor effects in the judgment of form. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 589–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, E. D., and Bevan, W. Patterns of experience and the perceived rotation of the Necker cube. Journal of General Psychology, 1964, 70, 345–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, E. D., and Bevan, W. The perception of auditory patterns as a function of incidental visual stimulation. Psychonomic Science 1964b, 1 135–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Wright, J. M., and Mikkonen, V. Changes in repeated reproduction of weight as a function of adaptation level. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1964, 5, 239–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vourinen, R. Effects of series and anchor weights on adaptation-level. Reports from the Institute of Psychology, University of Helsinki, No. 2, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vourinen, R. The concept of adaptation-level in psychological research. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1973, 14, 228–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, K., and Avant, L. L. Anchoring stimuli and Titchener’s illusion. U.S. Army Human Engineering Laboratories Technical Note No. 7–70, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, W. A., Jr., and Wilson, M. Physiological psychology: Neuropsychology. In H. Helson and W. Bevan (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to psychology. Princeton, N. J.: Van Nostrand, 1967, pp. 35–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeiler, M. D. The ratio theory of intermediate size discrimination. Psychological Review, 1963a, 70, 516–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeiler, M. D. New dimensions of the intermediate size problem: Neither absolute nor relational response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963b, 66, 588–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1978 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bevan, W., Gaylord, S. (1978). Stimuli, the Perceiver, and Perception. In: Walk, R.D., Pick, H.L. (eds) Perception and Experience. Perception and Perceptual Development, vol 1. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-2619-9_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-2619-9_12

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-2621-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4684-2619-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics