Advertisement

The Recording of Licking Behavior

  • Jan A. W. M. Weijnen

Abstract

A contact-sensitive electronic instrument was described by Pilgrim (1948), but it was only after the publication of “An electronic drinkometer” (Hill and Stellar, 1951) that contact sensors became routine tools in studies requiring the recording of water intake. These devices are still commonly referred to as drinkometers; however, preference here will be given to the terms lick or contact sensors. The principle of the device is simple. One lead from the instrument makes contact with the drinking tube; the other one goes to the metal floor of the test chamber. Each time the animal completes the circuit, switching is accomplished in the output circuit of the sensor, and the switch can be used for counting or for some other means of registration.

Keywords

Test Chamber Current Intensity Plate Floor Contact Sensor Output Circuit 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allison, J., 1971, Microbehavioral features of nutritive and nonnutritive drinking in rats, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 76:408–417.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bintz, J., and Zucker, M., 1970, A multifunction pulseforming circuit, J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 13:161–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Campbell, B. A., and Masterson, F. A., 1969, Psychophysics of punishment, in “Punishment and Aversive Behavior” (B. A. Campbell and R. M. Church, eds.), Appleton-Century-Crofts, Meredith Corporation, New York, pp. 3–42.Google Scholar
  4. Campbell, B. A., and Teghtsoonian, R., 1958, Electrical and behavioral effects of different types of shock stimuli on the rat, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 51:185–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cone, A. L., and Cone, D. M., 1973, Variability in the burst lick rate of albino rats as a function of sex, time of day, and exposure to the test situation, Bull. Psychon. Soc. 2:283–284.Google Scholar
  6. Corbit, J. D., and Luschei, E. S., 1969, Invariance of the rat’s rate of drinking, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 69:119–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Crawford, M. L. J., 1970, Shock-avoidance and shock-escape drinking in rats: Rate of licking, Psychon. Sci. 21:304–305.Google Scholar
  8. Davis, J. D., 1973, The effectiveness of some sugars in stimulating licking behavior in the rat, Physiol. Behav. 11:39–45.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eisman, E., 1969, Monitoring drinking: A method compatible with the recording of electrophysiological phenomena, Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 1:300–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hill, J. H., and Stellar, E., 1951, An electronic drinkometer, Science 114:43–44.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hulse, S. H., 1967, Licking behavior of rats in relation to saccharin concentration and shifts in fixed-ratio reinforcement, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 64:478–484.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hutcheson, J. S., and Mills, K. C., 1974, A compact and inexpensive drinkometer for use with small animals, Physiol. Behav. 13:179–181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. King, N. W., 1969, Effects of low-level microwave irradiation upon reflexive, operant, and discrimination behaviors of the rat, doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas.Google Scholar
  14. King, N. W., Justesen, D. R., and Simpson, A. D., 1970, The photo-lickerandum: A device for detecting the licking response, with capability for near-instantaneous programming of variable quantum reinforcement, Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 2:125–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Marowitz, L. A., and Halpern, B. P., 1973, The effects of environmental constraints upon licking pattern, Physiol. Behav. 11:259–263.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Martonyi, B., and Valenstein, E. S., 1971, On drinkometers: Problems and an inexpensive photocell solution, Physiol. Behav. 7:913–914.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Oatley, K., and Dickinson, A., 1970, Air drinking and the measurement of thirst, Anim. Behav. 18:259–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Pilgrim, F. J., 1948, A simple electronic relay for counting, timing, or automatic control, J. Psychol. 26:537–540.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ramsay, D. A., Knapp, J. Z., and Zeiss, J. C., 1970, Transients in “constant-current” generators, Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. 2:122–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Segal, E. F., and Oden, D. L., 1969, Effects of drinkometer current and of foot shock on psychogenic polydipsia, Psychon. Sci. 14:13–14.Google Scholar
  21. Slangen, J. L., and Weijnen, J. A. W. M., 1972, The reinforcing effect of electrical stimulation of the tongue in thirsty rats, Physiol. Behav. 8:565–568.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Vrtunski, P., and Wolin, L. R., 1974, Measurement of licking response execution in the rat, Physiol. Behav. 12:881–886.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Weijnen, J. A. W. M., 1972, Lick-contingent electrical stimulation of the tongue; its reinforcing properties in rats under dipsogenic conditions, doctoral dissertation, University of Utrecht (University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, Mich. Order No. 76–6040).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1977

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jan A. W. M. Weijnen
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Physiological Psychology SectionTilburg UniversityTilburgThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations