Advertisement

The Ontogeny of Cerebral Dominance

  • Marcel Kinsbourne

Abstract

It has long been known that in most people the language function depends on the activity of the left cerebral hemisphere. It is clear that language develops from a base state of no language. But does language lateralization analogously develop from a base state of no lateralization? It has been generally assumed that this is so. The child at the very beginning of language development is regarded as using both cerebral hemispheres for this purpose. Differences of opinion have related more to the time frame within which lateralization occurs (Lenneberg, 1967; Krashen, 1972) than to the question of whether lateralization develops at all.

Keywords

Brain Damage Language Function Dichotic Listening Head Turning Language Disorder 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alajouanine, T., and Lhermitte, F. Acquired aphasia in children. Brain, 1965, 88, 653–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Basser, L. Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain, 1962, 85, 427–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlin, C. I., Lowe-Bell, S. S., Hughes, L. F., and Berlin, H. L. Dichotic right-ear advantage in males and females—ages 5-13. Paper read at the 84th meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Miami Beach, Florida, 1972Google Scholar
  4. Bryden, M. P. Laterality effect in dichotic listening. Relations with handedness and reading ability in children. Neuropsychologia, 1970, 8, 443–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Byers, R. K., and McLean, W. T. Etiology and course of certain hemiplegias with aphasia in childhood. Pediatrics, 1962, 129, 367–383.Google Scholar
  6. Caplan, P., and Kinsbourne, M. Baby drops the rattle. Asymmetry of duration of grasp by infants. Child Development, 1976, 47, 532–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carlson, J., Netley, C., Hendrick, E. B., and Prichard, J. S. A reexamination of intellectual disabilities in hemispherectomized patients. Transactions of the American Neurological Association, 1968, 93, 198–201.Google Scholar
  8. Entus, A., Hemispheric asymmetry in processing of dichotically presented speech and non-speech sounds by infants. Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, Colorado, 1975.Google Scholar
  9. Eustis, R. Specific reading disability: A familial syndrome associated with ambidexterity and speech defects and a frequent cause of problem behavior. New England Journal of Medicine, 1947, 237, 243–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gefner, D. S., and Hochberg, I. Ear laterality preference of children from low and middle socio-economic levels on a verbal dichotic listening task. Cortex 1971, 7, 193–203.Google Scholar
  11. Geschwind, N. Neurological foundations of language. In H. R. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (Vol. 1). New York: Grune & Stratton, 1967.Google Scholar
  12. Guttmann, E. Aphasia in children. Brain, 1942, 65, 205–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hecaen, H., and Ajuriaguerra, J. Left handedness: Manual superiority and cerebral dominance. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1964.Google Scholar
  14. Kimura, D. Right temporal lobe damage. Archives of Neurology, 1963, 8, 264–271. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kimura, D. Speech lateralization in young children as determined by an auditory test. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 1963, 56, 899–902. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kinsbourne, M. Eye and head turning indicates cerebral lateralization. Science, 1972, 176, 539–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kinsbourne, M. Mechanisms of hemispheric interaction in man. In M. Kinsbourne and W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas, 1974.Google Scholar
  18. Kinsbourne, M. The interaction between cognitive process and the direction of attention. In V. P. M. A. Rabbitt and S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance. London: Academic Press, 1975. 5, 81–97.Google Scholar
  19. Kinsbourne, M., and Cook, J. Generalized and lateralized effects of concurrent verbalization on a unimanual skill. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 23, 341–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kinsbourne, M., and Hiscock, M. Does cerebral dominance develop? In S. Segalowitz and F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory. New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  21. Kinsbourne, M., and McMurray, J. The effect of cerebral dominance on time sharing between speaking and tapping by preschool children. Child Development, 1975, 46, 240–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kohn, B., and Dennis, M. Patterns of hemispheric specialization after hemidecortication for infantile hemiplegia. In M. Kinsbourne and W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas, 1974. Pp. 39–47.Google Scholar
  23. Krashen, S. Language and the left hemisphere. UCLA working papers in phonetics, 1972, 24.Google Scholar
  24. Lansdell, H. Verbal and nonverbal factors in right-hemisphere speech: Relation to early neurological history. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 1969, 69, 734–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lenneberg, E. Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1967.Google Scholar
  26. McFie, J. The effects of hemispherectomy on intellectual functioning in cases of infantile hemiplegia. Journal of Nurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 1961, 24, 240–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Molfese, D. L. Cerebral asymmetry in infants, children and adults: Auditory evoked responses to speech and musical stiumuli. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1973, 53, 363(A).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nagafuchi, M. Development of dichotic and monaural hearing abilities in young children. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 1970, 69, 409–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Orton, S. Reading, writing and speech problems in children. New York: Norton, 1937.Google Scholar
  30. Silver, A., and Hagin, R. Specific reading disability: Follow-up studies. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1964, 34, 95–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Siqueland, E. R., and Lipsitt, L. P. Conditioned head turning in human newborns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 4, 356–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Smith, A. Dominant and nondominant hemispherectomy. In M. Kinsbourne and W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas, 1974. Pp. 5–33.Google Scholar
  33. Turkewitz, G., Gordon, B. W., and Birch, M. G. Head turning in the human neonate: Effect of prandial condition and lateral preference. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 1968, 59, 189–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wilson, P. J. E. Cerebral hemispherectomy for infantile hemiplegia. Brain, 1970, 93, 147–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Zangwill, O. L. Cerebral dominance and its relation to psychological function. Edinburgh, Scotland: Oliver & Boyd, 1960.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marcel Kinsbourne
    • 1
  1. 1.Hospital for Sick ChildrenTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations