Advertisement

Some Fundamental Approaches to an Analysis of Limbic System Function

  • Leonard W. Hamilton

Abstract

In the preceding chapters it became apparent that the anatomical connections of the limbic system are so complex in their interactions that it is virtually impossible to view them as an integrated system. The original definition of the limbic system, as outlined by Broca in 1878, considered the limbic lobe to consist primarily of the septum, the cingulate cortex, and the hippocampal complex (refer to Fig. 1-1). Because of their seemingly close relationship with primary olfactory structures, the prevailing view at that time was that the limbic lobe was involved with higher-order olfactory processing. However, as anatomical methods became more precise, it was found that the olfactory bulbs did not contribute heavily to these structures (cf. Pribram & Kruger, 1954). These data, along with the classic paper by Papez (1937) suggesting a visceromotor function for these structures, led to a disuse of the term “rhinencephalon.”

Keywords

Limbic System Behavioral Inhibition Septal Lesion Fundamental Approach Ablation Method 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Albert, D.J. A simple method of making cuts in brain tissue. Physiol. Behay. 4: 863–864, 1969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anand, B.K., and Brobeck, J.R. Hypothalamic control of food intake in rats and cats. Yale J. Biol. Med., 24: 123–140, 1951.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beatty, W.W., and Schwartzbaum, J.S. Enhanced reactivity to quinine and saccharin solutions following septal lesions in the rat. Psychon. Sci., 8: 483–484, 1968.Google Scholar
  4. Bitterman, M.E. Phyletic differences in learning. Am. Psychol., 20: 396–410, 1965.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Broca, P. Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales. Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure limbique dans laserie des mammifères. Rev. Anthropol., 1: 385–498, 1878.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, G., and Remley, N. The effects of septal and olfactory bulb lesions on stimulus reactivity. Physiol. Behay., 6: 497–501, 1971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bures, J., Buresova, O., and Fifkova, E. Interhemispheric transfer of a passive avoidance reaction. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 57: 326–330, 1964.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Campbell, B.A., Lytle, L.D., and Fibiger, H.C. Ontogeny of adrenergic arousal and cholinergic inhibitory mechanisms in the rat. Science, 166: 635–637, 1969.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carlton, P.L. Cholinergic mechanisms in the control of behavior by the brain. Psychol. Rev. 40:19–39, 1963. Carlton, P.L. Brain acetylcholine and inhibition. In Tapp, J. (Ed.): Reinforcement and Behavior, New York, Academic Press, pp. 288–325, 1969.Google Scholar
  10. Carlton, P.L., and Advokat, C. Attenuated habituation due to parachlorophenylalanine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behay., 1: 657–663, 1974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark, G. The use of the Horsely-Clark instrument on the rat. Science, 90: 92, 1939.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cooper, J.R., Bloom, F.E., and Roth, R.H. The Biochemical Basis of Neuropharmacology. New York, Oxford University Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  13. Crow, T.J. Catecholamine containing neurons and electrical self-stimulation. 1. A review of some data. Psychol. Med., 2: 414–421, 1972.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Feigley, D.A. Effects of scopolamine on activity and passive avoidance learning in rats of different ages. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 87: 26–36, 1974.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Feigley, D.A., Parsons, P.J., Hamilton, L.W., and Spear, N.E. Development of habituation to novel environments in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 79: 443–452, 1972.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fritsch, G., and Hitzig, E. Über die elektrische Erregbarkeit des Grosshirns. Arch. Anat. Physiol. Wissen. Med., 37: 300–332, 1870.Google Scholar
  17. Goodman, L.S., and Gilman, A. The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. New York. Macmillan, 1965. Gottlieb, G. Ontogenesis of sensory function in birds and mammals. In Tobach, E., Aronson, L.R., and Shaw, E. (Eds.): Biopsychology of Development, New York. Academic Press, pp. 67–128, 1971.Google Scholar
  18. Grossman, S.P. Behavioral effects of direct chemical stimulation of central nervous system structures. Mt. J. Neuropharmacol., 3: 45–58, 1964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Grossman, S.P. A Textbook of Physiological Psychology. New York, Wiley, 1967.Google Scholar
  20. Halasz, B., and Pupp, L. Hormone secretion of the anterior pituitary gland after physical interruption of all nervous pathways to the hypophysiotropic area. Endocrinology, 77: 553–562, 1965.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hamilton, L.W., and Timmons, C.R. Knife cuts while you wait: a simple and inexpensive procedure for producing knife cuts in freely moving animals. Physiol. Behay., 16: 101–103, 1976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hamilton, L.W., Capobianco, S., and Worsham, E. Lowered response to postingestive cues following septal lesions in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 87: 134–141, 1974.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hamilton, L.W., Worsham, E., and Capobianco, S. A spring-loaded carrier for transection of fornix and other large fiber bundles. Physiol. Behay., 10: 157–159, 1973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harlow, H.F., Blomquist, A.J., Thompson, C.I., Schiltz, K.A., and Harlow, M.K. Effects of induction age and size of frontal lobe lesions on learning in rhesus monkeys. In Isaacson, R.L. (Ed.): The Neuropsychology of Development, New York, Wiley, pp. 79–120, 1968.Google Scholar
  25. Hetherington, A.W., and Ranson, S.W. Hypothalamic lesions and adiposity in the rat. Anat. Rec., 78: 149, 1940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Horsely, V., and Clark, R.H. The structure and functions of the cerebellum examined by a new method. Brain, 31: 45–124, 1908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Isaacson, R.L. The Limbic System, New York, Plenum Press, 1974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Isaacson, R.L., Nonneman, A.J., and Schmaltz, L.W. Behavioral and anatomical sequelae of the infant limbic system. In Isaacson, R.L. (Ed.): The Neuropsychology of Development, New York, Wiley, pp. 41–78, 1968.Google Scholar
  29. Jouvet, M. The role of monoamines and acetylcholine-containing neurons in the regulation of the sleep-waking cycle. Ergeb. Physiol., 64: 166–307, 1972.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kaada, B.R. Somato-motor, autonomic and electroencephalographic responses to electrical stimulation of “rhinencephalic” and other forebrain structures in primates, cat and dog. Acta Physiol. Scand., 83: 1–285, 1951.Google Scholar
  31. Friedhoff, A.J., Catecholamines and Behavior, New York, Plenum Press, pp. 59–87, 1975.Google Scholar
  32. MacLean, P.D. Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies of fronto-temporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain). Electoeneephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol., 4:407–418, 1952Google Scholar
  33. MacLean, P.D. The triune brain, emotion and scientific bias. In Schmidt, F.O. (Ed.): The Neurosciences Second Study Program, New York, Rockefeller University Press, pp. 336–348, 1970.Google Scholar
  34. Mabry, P.D., and Campbell, B.A. Ontogeny of serotonergic inhibition of behavioral arousal in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 86: 193–201, 1974.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mandel, P., Mack, G., and Goridis, C. Function of the central catecholaminergic neuron: synthesis, release, and inactivation of the transmitter. In Friedhoff, A.J. (Ed.): Catecholamines and Behavior, New York, Plenum Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  36. McCleary, R.A. Response specificity in the behavioral effects of limbic system lesions in the cat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 54: 605–613, 1961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McCleary, R.A. Response-modulating functions of the limbic system: initiation and suppression. In Stellar, E., and Sprague, J.M. (Eds.): Progress in Physiological Psychology, Vol. 1. New York, Academic Press, 1966. McLennan, H. Synaptic Transmission. Philadelphia, Saunders, 1970.Google Scholar
  38. Milner, P.M. Physiological Psychology. New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1970.Google Scholar
  39. Myers, R.D. Methods in Psychobiology. London, Academic, 1972.Google Scholar
  40. Nauta, W.J.H. Hippocampal projections and related neural pathways to the midbrain in the cat. Brain, 81: 319340, 1958.Google Scholar
  41. Nauta, W.J.H. Connections of the frontal lobe with the limbic system. In Laitinen, L. V., and Livingston, K.E. (Eds.): Surgical Approaches in Psychiatry, Baltimore, University Park Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  42. Papez, J.W. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 38: 725–743, 1937.Google Scholar
  43. Penfield, W., and Rasmussen, T. The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York, Macmillan, 1950.Google Scholar
  44. Pribram, K.H., and Kruger, L. Functions of the “olfactory brain.” Ann. N.Y. Acad Sci., 58: 109–138, 1954.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Robinson, B.W. Forebrain alimentary responses: some organizational principles. In Wayner, M.J. (Ed.): Thrist First International Symposium on Thirst in the Regulation of Body Water, New York, Pergamon, 1964.Google Scholar
  46. Robinson, B.W. Forebrain alimentary responses: some organizational principles. In Wayner, M.J. (Ed.): Thrist First International Symposium on Thirst in the Regulation of Body Water, New York, Pergamon, 1964.Google Scholar
  47. Schoenfeld, T.A., Hamilton, L.W., and Gandelman, R. Septal damage during the maturation of inhibitory responding: effects in juvenile and adult rats. Devel. Psychobiol., 7: 195–205, 1974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sclafani, A., and Grossman, S.P. Hyperphagia produced by knife cuts between the medial and lateral hypothalamus in the rat. Physiol. Behay., 4: 533–537, 1969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Shute, C.C.D., and Lewis, P.R. The ascending cholinergic reticular system: neocortical, olfactory and subcortical projections. Brain, 90: 497–522, 1967.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Stein, L. Chemistry of reward and punishment. In Efron, D.H. (Ed.): Psychopharmacology: A Review of Progress, 1957–1967, U.S.P.H.S., pp. 105–123, 1968.Google Scholar
  51. Wiess, B., and Laties, V.G. Enhancement of human performance by caffeine and the amphetamines. Pharmacol. Rev., 14: 1–1962.Google Scholar
  52. Williams, J.M., Hamilton, L.W., and Carlton, P.L. An ontogenetic analysis of two classes of habituation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 89: 733–737, 1975.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. White, B.C. Effects of hippocampal lesions on reversal and passive avoidance learning: an ontogenetic study. A paper presented to the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, Massachusetts, 1972.Google Scholar
  54. White, B.C. Effects of hippocampal lesions on reversal and passive avoidance learning: an ontogenetic study. A paper presented to the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, Massachusetts, 1972.Google Scholar
  55. White, B.C., & McGee, S.A. Personal communication.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • Leonard W. Hamilton
    • 1
  1. 1.Rutgers UniversityNew BrunswickUSA

Personalised recommendations