Advertisement

Reductionism, Levels of Organization, and the Mind-Body Problem

  • William C. Wimsatt

Abstract

Dr. Sperry’s position is complex and variegated, and I will be unable to discuss some of the issues raised by him in this volume and in other closely related papers. He has chosen to characterize his position generally as “emergent interactionism” and to spend a great deal of time distinguishing it from other views, ranging over the spectrum from dualism to materialism. His more general conception justifies and merits a more general response, which I shall present here.

Keywords

Explanatory Primacy Downward Causation Explanatory Priority Lower Level Theory Identity Thesis 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bartlett, F. C., and John, E. R. (1973): Equipotentiality quantified: The anatomical distribution of the engram. Science 181, pp. 764–767.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Blachowitz, J. A. (1971): Systems theory and evolutionary models of the development of Science. Philos. Sci. 38, pp. 178–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Boyd, R. (1972): Determinism, laws, and predictability in principle. Philos. Sci. 39, 431–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bronowski, J. (1970): New concepts in the evolution of complexity: Stratified stability and unbounded plans. Sythese 21, pp. 228–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Campbell, D. T. (1958): Common fate, similarity, and other indices of the status of aggregates of persons as social entities. Behav. Sci. 3, 14–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Campbell, D. T. (1973fl): Evolutionary epistemology. In: The Philosophy of Karl Popper. Ed. by P. A. Schilpp. LaSalle, Illinois: Open Court, pp. 413–463.Google Scholar
  7. Campbell, D. T. (1974): “Downwards causation” in hierarchically organized biological systems. In: The Problem of Reduction in Biology. Ed. by F. Ayala and T. Dobzhansky, Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  8. Carlson, E. A. (1966): The Gene: A Critical History, Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
  9. Causey, R. (1972): Attribute-identities in microreductions. J. Philos. 69, pp. 407–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Darwin, C. (1872): The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Reprinted, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Darden, L. (1974): Reasoning in Scientific Change: The Field of Genetics at its Beginnings. Ph.D. dissertation. Committee on Conceptual Foundations of Science, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
  12. Dennett, D. C. (1969): Content and Consciousness. New York: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
  13. Dennett, D. C. (1971): Intentional systems. J. Philos. 68, pp. 87–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dewan, E. M. (1969): Cybernetics and attention. In: Attention in Neurophysiology. Ed. by C. R. Evans and T. B. Mulholland. London: Butterworths, pp. 323–347.Google Scholar
  15. Dresden, M. (1974): Reflections on fundamentally and complexity. In: Physical Reality and Mathematical Description ( Festschrift for Joseph Jauch ) Ed. by Charles Enz and Jagdit Mehra. Reidel: Dordrecht, pp. 133–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Feigl, H. (1967): The Mental and the Physical. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  17. Feyerabend, P. K. (1962): Explanation, reduction, and empiricism. In: Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Ed. by H. Feigl and G. Maxwell. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 28–97.Google Scholar
  18. Fodor, J. (1968): Psychological Explanation. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  19. Fodor, J. (1974): Special sciences (or: the disunity of science as a working hypothesis). Synthese 28, pp. 97–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garfinkel, A. (1975): Explanation and Individuals. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Philosophy, Harvard University.Google Scholar
  21. Globus, G. G. (1972): Biological foundations of the psychoneural identity hypothesis. Philos. Sci. 40, pp. 291–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Globus, G. G. (1973): Unexpected symmetries in the world knot. Science 180, pp. 1129–1137.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Glymour, C. (1973): Testability without meaning: An essay on distributive justice. Department of Philosophy, Princeton University (mimeo).Google Scholar
  24. Gregory, R. L. (1959): Models and the localization of function in the central nervous system. In: Key Papers: Cybernetics. Ed. by C. R. Evans and A. D. J. Robertson. London: Butterworths, pp. 91–102.Google Scholar
  25. Grunbaum, A. (1963): Philosophical Problems of Space and Time. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  26. Gunderson, K. (1970): Asymmetries and mind-body perplexities. In: Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science Vol 4. Ed. by M. Radner and S. Winokur. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 273–309.Google Scholar
  27. Gunderson, K. (1971): Mentality and Machines. Garden City: Doubleday (Anchor).Google Scholar
  28. Hanson, N. R. (1961): Patterns of Discovery. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Hanson, N. R. (1970): A picture theory of theory meaning. In: The Nature and Function of Scientific Theories. Ed. by R. G. Colodny, Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 233–274.Google Scholar
  30. Hull, D. L. (1972): Reduction in genetics—Biology or Philosophy? Philos. Sci. 39, pp. 491–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hull, D. L. (1973): Philosophy of Biology. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  32. Jeans, J. (1960): An Introduction to the Kinetic Theory of Gases. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Kim, J. (1964): Inference, explanation and prediction. J. Philos. 61, pp. 360–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kim, J. ( 1966: On the psycho-physical identity thesis. American Philosophical Quarterly 3, pp. 227–235.Google Scholar
  35. Kim, J. (1971): Materialism and the criteria of the mental. Synthese 22, pp. 323–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kuhn, T. A. (1962): The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (2nd ed., 1970 ).Google Scholar
  37. Levins, R. (1966): The strategy of model building in population biology. Am. Sci. 54, pp. 421–431.Google Scholar
  38. Levins, R. (1968): Evolution in Changing Environments. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Levins, R. (1970): Complex systems. In: Towards a Theoretical Biology, v. 3. Ed. by C. H. Waddington. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press, pp. 73–88.Google Scholar
  40. Levins, R. (1974): The qualitative analysis of partially specified systems. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 231, 123–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lewontin, R. C. (1970): Units of selection. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1, pp. 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Longuet-Higgins, H. C., Willshaw, D. J., and Buneman, O. P. (1970): Theories of associative recall. Quarterly Review of Biophysics. 3, pp. 223–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. McMahon, J. (1976): The Concept of Fitness in Evolutionary Biology. Ph.D. Dissertation. Department of Philosophy, University of Chicago (in preparation).Google Scholar
  44. McMullin, E. (1972): The dialectics of reduction. Idealistic Studies 2, pp. 95–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. H., and Pribram, K. H. (1960): Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nagel, E. (1961): The Structure of Science. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
  47. Nickles, T. (1973A): TWO concepts of intertheoretic reduction. J. Philos. 70, pp. 181–201.Google Scholar
  48. Nickles, T. (1973b): Reduction and Conceptual Change. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  49. Popper, K. (1959): The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
  50. Pribram, K. H. (1971a): Languages of the Brain. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  51. Rorty, R. (1965): Mind-body identity, privacy and categories. In: Modern Materialism: Readings on Mind-Body Identity. Ed. by J. O’Connor. New York: Harcourt, 1969, pp. 145–174.Google Scholar
  52. Rosenblatt, F. (1962a): Strategic approaches to the study of brain models. In: Principles of Self-Organization. Ed. by H. von Foerster and G. W. Zopf, New York: Pergamon Press, pp. 385–402.Google Scholar
  53. Rosenblatt, F. (1962b): Principles of Neurodynamics: Perceptions and the Theory of Brain Mechanisms, Spartan Books, Washington, D.C. (Also published by Cornell Aeronautical Laboratory as Report No. VG-1196-G-8, 15 March, 1961 ).Google Scholar
  54. Roth, N. (1974): Progress in Modern Biology: An Alternative to Reduction. Ph.D. dissertation. Committee on Conceptual Foundations of Science, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
  55. Salmon, W., Jeffrey, R., and Greeno, J. (1971): Statistical Explanation and Statistical Relevance. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
  56. Schaffner, K. F. (1967): Approaches to reduction. Philos. Sci. 34, pp. 137–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sellars, W. (1963): Philosophy and the scientific image of man. In: Science, Perception and Reality. Ed. by Sellars, London: Routledge, pp. 1–40.Google Scholar
  58. Shapere, D. (1966): Meaning and scientific change. In: Mind and Cosmos. Ed. R. G. Colodny, Pittsburgh, University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 41–85.Google Scholar
  59. Shapere, D. (1969): Notes towards a post-positivistic interpretation of science. In: The Legacy of Logical Positivism. Ed. by P. Achinstein and S. F. Barker, Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
  60. Shapere, D. (1974): Scientific theories and their domains. In: The Nature of Scientific Theories. Ed. by Frederick Suppe. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  61. Simon, H. A. (1957). A behavioral model of rational choice. In: Models of Man, Chapter 15. Ed. by Simon, New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  62. Simon, H. A. (1966a): Thinking by computers. In: Mind and Cosmos. Ed. by R. G. Colodny, Pittsburgh, University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 3–21.Google Scholar
  63. Simon, H. A. (1966b): Scientific discovery and the psychology of problem solving. In: Mind and Cosmos. Ed. by R. G. Colodny, Pittsburgh, The University of Pittsburgh Press, pp. 22–40Google Scholar
  64. Simon, H. A. (1969): The Sciences of the Artificial. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  65. Simon, H. A., and Chase, W. G. (1973): Skill in chess. Am. Sci. 61, pp. 394–403.Google Scholar
  66. Sklar, L. (1967): Types of inter-theoretic reduction. Br. J. Philos. Sci. 18, pp. 109–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sperry, R. W. (1966): Brain bisection and mechanisms of consciousness. In: Brain and Conscious Experience. Ed. by J. C. Eccles. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  68. Sperry, R. W. (1969): A modified concept of consciousness. Psychol. Rev. 76, pp. 532–536.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sperry, R. W. (1970): An objective approach to subjective experience: Further explanation of a hypothesis. Psychol. Rev. 77, pp. 585–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Sutton, W. S. (1903): The chromosomes in heredity. Biol. Bull. 4, 231–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Taylor, C. (1964): The Explanation of Behavior. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  72. Taylor, C. (1967): Mind-body identity, a side issue? Philos. Rev. 76, 201–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Thompson, D’Arcy W. (1961): On Growth and Form. Abridged edition, edited with commentary by J. T. Bonner. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  74. von Bekesy, G. (1967): Sensory Inhibition. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Waddington, C. H. (1970): Paradigm for an evolutionary process. In: Towards a Theoretical Biology Vol. 2. Ed. by Waddington. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press, pp. 106–124.Google Scholar
  76. Whittaker, R. H., Levin, S. A. and Root, R. B. (1973): Niche, habitat and ecotope. Am. Nat. 107, pp. 321–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Weisskopf, V. F. (1975): Of atoms, mountains and stars: A study in qualitative physics. Science 187, pp. 605–612.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Williams, G. C. (1966): Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  79. Wimsatt, W. C. (1970): review of G. C. Williams’ Adaptation and Natural Selection. Philos. Sci. 37, pp. 620–623.Google Scholar
  80. Wimsatt, W. C. (1971a): Modern Science and the New Teleology: I—The Conceptual Foundations of Functional Analysis. Ph.D. dissertation. Department of Philosophy, University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
  81. Wimsatt, W. C. (1971b): Aggregativity and complexity. In: Proceedings of the 4th International Congress for Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science. Ed. by P. Suppes.Google Scholar
  82. Wimsatt, W. C. (1972a): Teleology and the logical structure of function statements. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science. 3, pp. 1–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Wimsatt, W. C. (1972b): The machine in the ghost: The sciences of the Artificial. Invited commentary on the paper, Chance and Design in Darwin and in Neo-Darwinism, of Edward Manier at the 1972 Metaphysical Meetings (unpublished manuscript).Google Scholar
  84. Wimsatt, W. C. (1974a): Complexity and organization. In: Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science Vol. 20. Ed. by K. F. Schaffner and R. S. Cohen, Reidel: Dordrecht, pp. 67–86.Google Scholar
  85. Wimsatt, W. C. (1974b): The problem of spatiality in genetics and in the mental realm. In preparation.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1976

Authors and Affiliations

  • William C. Wimsatt
    • 1
  1. 1.University of ChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations