Advertisement

The Reactivity of Wistar Rats Highly Selected for Good and Bad Learning, Observed in Various Physiological and Pharmacological Test Models. 1st. Communication

  • H. Müller-Calgan
  • K. H. Becker
  • H. J. Enenkel
  • H. J. Schliep
  • A. J. N. Wild

Abstract

Preliminary investigations on selected rats, bred from Wistar WU/Ivanovas since 1966, presented the following results:
  1. 1.

    No differences could be detected between GL and BL as far as the pharmacological action on peripheral organs and receptors are concerned, viz. inflammation by means of Freund’s adjuvant, sensitivity to nor-adrenalin of the vascular system and sensitivity to carbachol and acetylcholine of cholinergic receptors in the gastrointestinal tract.

     
  2. 2.

    Without detectable differences in GL and BL were the effects of centrally acting substances such as myotonolytic tranquilizers, cataleptics, convulsants and narcotics.

     
  3. 3.

    Equally undifferentiated were pain sensitivity and exploration behaviour in both strains. These factors, therefore, do not come into consideration as causal agents of the differences in learning capacity of GL and BL.

     
  4. 4.

    Contrary to this, GL generally appeared to be slightly more sensitive to central stimulants and the sympathomimetic amphetamine, BL, however, distinctly more sensitive to the cholinergic tremorine(induced tremor). This is in accordance with the concepts of KHAVARI (1971) and LEITH and BARRETT (1971). KHAVARI assumes “implication of a dichotomous CNS adrenergic-cholinergic neurotransmitter mechanism in the control of learned behaviour”. LEITH and BARRETT conclude that “differences between strains in the avoidance performance are, at least partially, related to variations in the relative activity of the cholinergic and adrenergic systems”. Our own observations, however, demonstrated here a marked overlapping with the sex differences.

     
  5. 5.

    Distinct sex differences often exist.

     
  6. 5.1.

    The female animals are more sensitive to the chemical pain stimulant phenylbenzoquinone and explore slightly more frequently than the males. This could explain, at least in part, a difference in learning ability between the two sexes of the same strain.

     
  7. 5.2.

    In contradistinction to this, the females are more insensitive to two centrally acting muscle relaxants (at a higher level of the basic tonus), two narcotics and two cataleptics. A prominent sex difference was brought about by the cataleptic tetrabenazine. This sex difference could also be demonstrated in the control animals of two unselected strains. No sex differences in the narcotic action could be observed after hexobarbital administration. It is envisaged to extend our studies with GL and BL.

    The last contribution contains a literature survey as well as an extensive discussion on the results of our own investigations.

     

Keywords

Exploration Behaviour Pain Sensitivity Female Animal Avoidance Performance Avoidance Training 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literature

  1. Benesova, O., and V. Benes: The relation between the type of higher nervous activity, some biochemical parameters and the reactivity to drugs. Act. nerv. sup. 10, 223–231 (1968).Google Scholar
  2. Collins, T. B., Jr., and D. F. Lott: Stock and sex specifity in the response of rats to pentobarbital sodium. Labor. Anim. Care 18, 192–194 (1968).Google Scholar
  3. Courvoisier, S., R. Ducrot, and L. Julou: Psychotropic Drugs. In: S. Garattini and V. Ghetti), p. 373, Elsevier, Amsterdam 1957, cited by TaeschlerGoogle Scholar
  4. Craig, A. L., and H. J. Kupferberg: Heat loss and heat production in d-amphetamine hyperthermia in two strains of aggregated mice. Pharmacologist 13, 2: 306 (1971).Google Scholar
  5. Dixon, W. J., and A. M. Mood: A method for obtaining and analyzing sensitivity data. J. Amer. Statist. Assoc. 43, 109–126 (1948), and in Finney, D. J.: Staircase Estimation Probit Analysis, 2nd Ed., pp. 226–235 (1952).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fleckenstein, A.: Symposium International on Drugs and Metabolism of Myocardium and Striated Muscle. Ed. by M. Lamarche and R. Roye, Nancy 1969.Google Scholar
  7. Fuller, J.: Strain differences in the effects of chlorpromazine and chlordiazepoxide upon active and passive avoidance in mice. Psychopharmacologia 16, 261–271 (1970).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hillebrecht, J.: Zur routinemäßigen Prüfung antiphlogistischer Substanzen im Rattenpfotentest. Arzneimittel forsch. 4, 607–614 (1954).Google Scholar
  9. Hornykiewicz, O.: Dopamine (3-hydroxytyramine) and brain functions. Pharmacol. Reviews 18, 925–964 (1964).Google Scholar
  10. Isom, G. E., R. B. Nelson, and A. I. Edlin: A comparison of the lethal and respiratory effects of morphine in Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley rats. Arch. int. Pharmacodyn. 182, 130–138 (1969).Google Scholar
  11. Khavari, K. A.: Adrenergic-cholinergic involvement in modulation of learned behavior. J. comp, physiol. Psych. 74, 281–291 (1971).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kuhlenkampf, C., and G. Tarnow: Ein eigentümliches Syndrom im oralen Bereich bei Megaphenapplikation. Nervenarzt 27, 178 (1956).Google Scholar
  13. Leith, N. J., and R. J. Barrett: Relationship of adrenergic and cholinergic systems to strain differences in avoidance performance. Pharmacologist 13, 232 (1971).Google Scholar
  14. Magnus, R.: Versuche am überlebenden Dünndarm von Säugetieren. Arch. ges. Physiol. 102, 123–151 (1904).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Mann, Whitney: The Mann-Whitney U-Test. In: Nonparametric Statistics (S. S. Siegel, ed.), pp. 116–117, Mc. Graw Hill, Book Comp., New York (1956).Google Scholar
  16. Müller-Calgan, H: Teste des psychotropen Screening, Ratte (unveröffentlichte Methodensammlung).Google Scholar
  17. Müller-Calgan, H., S. Sommer, and H.-J. Jesdinsky: Bewertung psychotroperPharmaka an Ratten durch ED5Q-Schätzung mit der Auf- und Ab-Methode nach DIXON-MOOD. Naunyn-Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmak. exp. Path. 260, 178 (1968).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Müller-Calgan, H., and S. Sommer: Das Reserpin-Parkinsonoid beim Schimpansen und seine Behandlung mit Fencamfamin. Arch. Pharmak. exp. Path. 260, 177 (1968).Google Scholar
  19. Newbould, B. B.: Chemotherapy of arthritis induced in rats by mycobacterial adjuvant. Brit. J. Pharmacol. Chemother. 21, 127–136 (1963).Google Scholar
  20. Steinberg, H., R. Rushton, and C. Tinson: Modification of the effects of an amphetamine-barbiturate mixture by the past experience of rats. Nature 192, 533–535 (1961).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Stille, G.: Zur Pharmakologie katatonogener Stoffe. Editio Cantor, Aulendorf i. Württb. 1971, Arzneimittelforsch. 21, (1.–6. Mitteilung), 225; 997 (1971).Google Scholar
  22. Selye, H.: The pluricausal cardiopathies. Ch. C. Thoma Publ. Springfield/Ill., p. 114 (1961).Google Scholar
  23. Shipley, R. E., and J. H. Tilden: Pithed rat preparation suitable for essaying pressor substances. Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. Med. 64, 453–455 (1967).Google Scholar
  24. Schlesinger, K., W. O. Boggan, and B. J. Griek: Pharmaco-genetic correlates of pentylentetrazol and electro-convulsive seizure thresholds in mice. Psychopharmacologia 13, 181–188 (1968).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Taeschler, M., H. Weidmann, and A. Cerletti: Zur Pharmakologie von Pönal id, einem neuen zentralen Anticholinergikum. Schweiz. Med. Wschr. 92, 1542–1545 (1962).Google Scholar
  26. Wilcoxon, F. in Siegel, S. S.: The Wilcoxon-Matched-pairs signed-ranks test. Mc. Graw Hill Book Comp., New York, pp. 75–83 (1956).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1973

Authors and Affiliations

  • H. Müller-Calgan
    • 1
  • K. H. Becker
    • 1
  • H. J. Enenkel
    • 1
  • H. J. Schliep
    • 1
  • A. J. N. Wild
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Pharmacology, Medical Research DivisionE. Merck61 DarmstadtGermany

Personalised recommendations