Advertisement

Studies with Dark Avoidance and Scotophobin

  • Helene N. Guttman
  • G. Matwyshyn
  • M. Weiler

Abstract

Stability of scotophobin, 5,11-deamidoscotophobin and N-acetyl 5,11-deamidoscotophobin is compared using micro-dansylation followed by chromatographic analysis.

Methods for the routine testing and analysis of results of dark avoidance experiments are considered. The methods allow analysis of results with animals which vary with respect to reaction initiation time, and duration of reaction.

Using the paradigm of dark avoidance activity analysis followed by dark-avoidance training, it is shown that there is low-grade, variable and transient dark avoidance activity for the following compounds: 5,11-deamidoscotophobin, N-acetyl 5,11-deamidoscotophobin, 8-15 amino acid fragment of deamidoscotophobin, and degraded scotophobin.

Proof is presented that scotophobin passes the blood-brain barrier.

Scotophobin does not exert its action by causing a general reduction of activity of goldfish.

Participation of various amino acid residues of scotophobin in binding and activity is discussed. During manifestation of dark avoidance activity, scotophobin is bound to a large molecular weight cellular site. Synaptic membranes or S-100 proteins are candidates for binding materials.

Theoretical considerations on relationship of induction of behavioral alteration and of other inducible, biological processes are briefly considered.

Keywords

Passive Transfer Saving Activity Amino Acid Fragment Passive Reaction Vagal Lobe 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literature

  1. Ali, A., J. H. R. Faesel, D. Sarantakis, D. Stevenson, and B. Weinstein: Synthesis of a structure proposed for scotophobin. Experientia 27, 1138–1139 (1971).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Daniels, P.: Acquisition, storage, and recall of memory for brightness discrimination by rats following intra-cerebral infusion of acetoxycyclohexamide. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 76, 110–118 (1971).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Desiderio, D. M., G. Ungar, and P. A. White: The use of mass spectrometry in the structural elucidation of scotophobia, a specific behavior-inducing brain peptide. Chem. Commun. 9, 432–433 (1971).Google Scholar
  4. Golub, A. M., and J. V. Mcconnell: Empirical issues in inter-animal transfer of information. In: Chemical Transfer of Learned Information (E. J. Fjerdingstad, ed.), pp. 1–29, North-Holland, Amsterdam (1971).Google Scholar
  5. Gray, W. R., and B. S. Hartley: A fluorescent end-group reagent for proteins and peptides. Biochem. J. 89, 59 (1963).Google Scholar
  6. Guttman, H. N., and L. Gronke: Passive transfer of learned dark and step-down avoidance. Psychon. Sci. 24, 107–109 (1971).Google Scholar
  7. Guttman, H. N., G. Matwyshyn, and G. H. Warriner III: Synthetic scotophobin-mediated passive transfer of dark avoidance. Nature New Biology 235, 26–27 (1972).Google Scholar
  8. Jacobson, A. L., F. R. Bubash, and S. Jacobson: Differential approach tendencies produced by injection of RNA from trained rats. Science 150, 636–637 (1965).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Katz, M. S., and W. C. Halstead: Protein organization and mental function. Comp. Psych. Monogr. 20, 1–38 (1950).Google Scholar
  10. Kesner, R. P., and, H. S. Conner: Independence of short- and long-term memory: a neural system analysis. Science 176, 432–4, (1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Luttgers, M., T. Johnson, C. Burk, J. Holland, and J. Mcgaugh: An examination of “transfer of learning” by nucleic acid. Science 151, 834–837 (1966).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Neuhoff, V., F. Von Der Haas, E. Schlimme, and M. Weise: Zweidimensionale Chromatographie von Dansyl-Aminsäuren im pico-Mol-Bereich, angewandt zur direkten Charakterisierung von Transfer-Ribonucleinsäuren. Hoppe-Seyler’s Z. Physiol. Chem. 350, 121–128 (1969).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Parr, W., and G. Holzer: Synthesen von Scotophobin-analoga. Hoppe-Seyler’s Z. Physiol. Chem. 352, 1043–1047 (1971).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Russell, R. W., G. Ungar, and E. Usdin: Seminar on the requirements for testing of hypotheses about molecular coding of experience: transfer studies. Psychopharm. Bull. 8, 5–13 (1972).Google Scholar
  15. Szilard, L.: On memory and recall. Proc. Soc. Nat Acad. Sci. Wash. 51, 1092–1099 (1964).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ungar, G.: Role of proteins and peptides in learning and memory. In: Molecular Mechanisms in Memory and Learning (G. Ungar, ed.), pp. 149–175, Plenum, New York (1970).Google Scholar
  17. Ungar, G., and S. R. Burzynski: Detection of a behavior-inducing peptide (scotophobin) in brain by ultramicro-analytical method. Fed. Proc. 31, 398 (1972).Google Scholar
  18. Ungar, G., and E. J. Fjerdingstad: Chemical nature of the transfer factors; RNA or protein? In: Biology of Memory (G. Adam, ed), pp. 137–143, Plenum, New York (1971).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1973

Authors and Affiliations

  • Helene N. Guttman
    • 1
  • G. Matwyshyn
    • 1
  • M. Weiler
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Biological SciencesUniversity of Illinois at Chicago CircleChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations