Advertisement

Conditions for Retroactive Inhibition

  • A. A. Smirnov
Part of the Monographs in Psychology book series (MOPSY)

Abstract

Retroactive inhibition is the negative effect of an activity following memorization on the retention of the material memorized. If memorization is followed by some other activity, recall of the material may not be as complete as when the memorization is followed by rest. This deterioration of recall is due to the “inhibitory” effect of the activity following the memorization. Therefore, the inhibition involved has been designated retroactive inhibition. Obviously, this “retroactive effect” cannot be interpreted literally as an influence on the actual process of memorization. By the time the next activity begins, this process has already been completed, and therefore we are dealing not with an influence on the process itself, but only on the “traces” resulting from the memorization.

Keywords

Subsequent Activity Neural Element Preceding Activity Nonsense Syllable Subsequent Memorization 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Bibliography

  1. Britt (1935), “Retroactive inhibition,” Psychol. Bull., Vol. 32.Google Scholar
  2. Cheng, N. (1929), “Retroactive effect and degree of similarity,” J. Exper. Psychol., Vol. 12.Google Scholar
  3. De-Camp, J. (1935), “A study of retroactive inhibition,” Psychol. Monog., No. 84.Google Scholar
  4. Dreis, T. (1933), “Two studies in retroaction,” J. Gen. Psychol., Vol. 8.Google Scholar
  5. Gengerelli (1934), “Similarity and retroaction,” J. Exper. Psychol., Vol. 17.Google Scholar
  6. Harden, L. (1929), “A quantitative study of the similarity factor in retroactive inhibition,” J. Gen. Psychol., Vol. 2.Google Scholar
  7. Jenkins, J. and K. Dallenbach (1924), “Obliviscence during sleep and waking,” Amer. J. Psychol., Vol. 35.Google Scholar
  8. Johnson, L. (1933), “Similarity of meaning as a factor in retroactive inhibition,” J. Gen. Psychol., Vol. 9.Google Scholar
  9. McGeoch, J. (1932), “Forgetting and the law of disuse,” Psychol. Rev., Vol. 39.Google Scholar
  10. McGeoch, J. and W. McDonald (1931), “Meaningful relation and retroactive inhibition,” Amer. J. Psychol., 43:579–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McGeoch, J. and F. McKinney (1934a), “Retroactive inhibition in the learning of poetry,” Amer. J. Psychol., 46:19–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. McGeoch, J. and F. McKinney (1934b), “The susceptibility of prose to retroactive inhibition,” Amer. J. Psychol., 46:429–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Müller, G. and A. Pilzecker (1900), Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre von Gedächtnis.Google Scholar
  14. Robinson, E. (1920), “Some factors determining the degree of retroaction,” Psychol Monog., No. 128.Google Scholar
  15. Robinson, E. (1927), “The similarity factor in retroaction,” Amer. J. Psychol., Vol. 39.Google Scholar
  16. Skaggs, E. (1925), “Further studies in retroactive inhibition,” Psychol Monog., No. 34.Google Scholar
  17. Stöiring (1931), Über den ersten reinen Fall eines Menschen mit völligem isolierten Verlust der Merkfähigkeit,” Arch. f. d. ges. Psychol., Vol. 81.Google Scholar
  18. Thorndike, E. (1923), Educational Psychology, Vol. 2.Google Scholar
  19. Webb, L. (1917), “Transfer of Training and Retroaction,” p. 104, summarized in: Britt (1935), “Retroactive inhibition,” Psychol. Bull., Vol. 32.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1973

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. A. Smirnov
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Institute of PsychologyAcademy of Pedagogical SciencesMoscowRussia
  2. 2.Moscow State University and Moscow City Pedagogical InstituteRussia

Personalised recommendations