Advertisement

Adaptation to Equilibration: A More Complex Model of the Applications of Piaget’s Theory to Early Childhood Education

  • Christine Chaillé
Part of the NATO Conference Series book series (NATOCS, volume 14)

Abstract

Three levels of educational applications of Piagetian theory are delineated in this paper--the level of the learning experiences of the child; the level of the teacher’s views of teaching and learning; and the level of the transmission of culture through the educational process. Each of these levels has been approached with a simple interactionist model of equilibration. Although this model has been a fruitful one and has generated considerable thinking and research, it may rest on oversimple epistemological assumptions. The problems that need to be addressed are restated given the more complex model of equilibration set forth by Piaget (1977).

Keywords

Educational Application Cultural Reproduction Early Childhood Education Program Childhood Education Program Considerable Thinking 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bourdieu, P. and Passeron, J. Reproduction in education, society and culture. Beverly Hills, California: Sage Productions, 1977.Google Scholar
  2. Chaillé, C. The child’s conceptions of play, pretending, and toys: Sequences and structural parallels. Human Development, 1978, 21, 201–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chaillé, C. Adaptation and equilibration: An essay review of Jean Piaget, The Development of Thought and The Grasp of Consciousness. Harvard Educational Review, 1979, 101–106.Google Scholar
  4. Chains, C. and Young, P. Some issues linking research on children’s play and education: Are they “only playing”? International Journal of Early Childhood, in press.Google Scholar
  5. Duckworth, E. The having of wonderful ideas. Harvard Educational Review, 1972, 217–231.Google Scholar
  6. Forman, G., and Kuschner, D. The child’s construction of knowledge: Piaget for teaching children. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company, 1977.Google Scholar
  7. Greenfield, P. M. Structural parallels between language and action in development. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, Gesture and Symbol: The Emergence of Language. New York: Academic Press, 1978. Hunt, J. McV. Intelligence and Experience. New York: Ronald Press, 1961.Google Scholar
  8. Kamii, C., and DeVries, R. Piaget, Children, and Number. National Association for the Education of Young Children, 1976.Google Scholar
  9. Kamii, C., and DeVries, R. Physical Knowledge in Preschool Education: Implications of Piaget’s Theory. New York: Prentice Hall, 1978.Google Scholar
  10. Karmiloff-Smith, A., and Inhelder, B. “If you want to get ahead, get a theory.” Cognition, 1975, 3, 195–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Piaget, J. The Grasp of Consciousness: Action and Concept in the Young Child. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  12. Piaget, J. The Development of Thought: Equilibration of Cognitive Structures. New York: The Viking Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  13. Pinard, A., and Laurendeau, M. “State” in Piaget’s cognitive-developmental theory: Exegenesis of a concept. In Elkind, D., and Flavell, J. (Eds.), Studies in Cognitive Development. New York: Oxford University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  14. Sigel, I. E. Constructivism and teacher education. The Elementary School Journal, 1978, 78, 333–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Weikart, D. P., Rogers, L., Adcock, C., and McClelland, D. The Cognitively Oriented Curriculum. ERIC, National Association for the Education of Young Children, 1971.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1981

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christine Chaillé
    • 1
  1. 1.University of ConnecticutStorrsUSA

Personalised recommendations