Skip to main content

Some Aspects of Sensory-Motor Control and Adaptation in Man

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Perception and Perceptual Development ((PPD))

Abstract

The control of human movement and spatial orientation is notably complex. The body is multiply articulate and as the trunk and limbs change their orientation with respect to the gravitational-force vector, the forces necessary to move a limb through a given angle also change. Moving the forearm back and forth through the same angular distances in a vertical and then in a horizontal plane represents a simple example of this. Even though the motor commands necessary to bring about the “same movement” in the two cases differ because of the load and orientation changes, one nevertheless, unless fatigued, experiences little or no difference in the effort required to bring about the movements. This means that the skeletomuscular system is “calibrated” such that body movements of a given extent are perceived as equivalent in terms of apparent force despite often radical differences in the actual forces involved. Such a calibration is possible only through a continual monitoring of the relative configuration and orientation of the body in relation to the substrate of support and the gravitoinertial-force vector. Actually, as Mach (1897) pointed out long ago, the eyes are really the only movable parts of the body that can be controlled by means of innervation sequences that bring about the same movements regardless of body orientation.1

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bahill, A., & Stark, L., The trajectories of saccadic eye movements. Scientific American, 1979, 240, 108–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, C., Introduction a l’Etude de la Médecine Expérimentale. Paris: J.B. Baillière et fils, 1865.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N., The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W.B., The wisdom of the body. New York: Norton, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Review of Verbal Behavior by B.F. Skinner (1957). Language, 1959, 35, 26–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewar, R., Adaptation to displaced vision: Variations on the “prismatic shaping” technique. Perception and Psychophysics, 1971, 9, 155–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks, G., Selective vocal effects of delayed auditory feedback. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1955, 20, 333–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R., The functional equivalence of mental images and errors of movement. Cognitive Psychology, 1979, 11, 235–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., & Walk, R., The “visual cliff.” Scientific American, 1960, 202, 64–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G., McCloskey D., & Matthews, P., The contribution of muscle afferents to kinesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. Brain, 1972, 95, 705–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel, A., Structural elements in the concept of motion sickness. Aerospace Medicine, 1969, 40, 351–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel, A., & Wood, C., Rapid vestibular adaptation in a rotating environment by means of controlled head movements. Aerospace Medicine, 1969, 40, 638–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel, A., Guedry, F., Johnson, W., & Kennedy, R., Adaptation to bizarre stimulation of the semicircular canals as indicated by the oculogyral illusion. Aerospace Medicine, 1961, 32, 321–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guedry, F., Psychophysics of vestibular sensation. In H. Kornhuber (Ed.), Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Volume VI/2: Vestibular System. Part 2: Psychophysics, Applied Aspects and General Interpretations. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1974, pp. 3–154.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hagbarth, K.-E., & Eklund, G., Motor affects of vibratory stimuli in man. In R. Granit (Ed.), Muscle Afferents and Motor Control. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell, 1966, pp. 177–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C., Adaptation to displaced vision: Visual, motor, or proprioceptive change. Science, 1963, 140, 812–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J., & Pick, H., Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71, 150–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1961, 132, 26–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., Plasticity in sensory-motor systems. Scientific American, 1965, 213, 84–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., Action contingent development of vision in neonatal animals. In D.P. Kimble (Ed.), Experience and Capacity. New York: New York Academy of Sciences, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Bossom, J., Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: Complementary techniques for analyzing plastic sensory-motor coordinations. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1961, 54, 33–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Freedman, S., Plasticity in human sensorimotor control. Science, 1963, 142, 455–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Gottlieb, N., Technique for studying adaptation to disarranged hand-eye coordination. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1958, 8, 83–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Hein, A., Adaptation to disarranged hand-eye coordination contingent upon reafferent stimulation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1958, 8, 87–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Hein, A., Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1963, 56, 872–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Schlank, M., Adaptation to disarranged eye-hand coordination in the distance dimension. American Journal of Psychology, 1959, 72, 603–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L.J. The fitness of the environment. New York: Macmillan, 1913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, T. Displacing the optic array. In S. Freedman (Ed.), The neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey Press, 1968, pp. 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D., Animals and men. London: Penguin Books, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D., & Lackner, J., Adaptation to delayed auditory feedback. Perception and Psychophysics, 1977, 22, 476–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kravitz, T., & Wallach, H., Adaptation to displaced vision contingent upon vibrating stimulation. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 6, 465–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., Influence of posture on the spatial localization of sounds. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., The role of posture in adaptation to visual rearrangement. Neuropsychologia, 1973, 11, 33–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., Adaptation to displaced vision: Role of proprioception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1974, 38, 1251–1256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., Induction of illusory self-rotation and nystagmus in stationary subjects with a rotating sound field. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1917, 48, 129–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., Some mechanisms underlying sensory and postural stability in man. In R. Held, H. Leibowitz, & H.L. Teuber (Eds.), Handbook of sensory physiology. Vol. 8: Perception. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1978, pp. 805–845.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., Adaptation to constancy of auditory direction transfers to visual direction. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Graybiel, A. Postural illusions experienced during z-axis recumbent rotation and their dependence upon somatosensory stimulation of the body surface. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1978, 49, 484–488. (a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Graybiel, A., Some influences of touch and pressure cues on human spatial orientation. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1978, 49, 798-804. (b)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Graybiel, A., Relationship between slow-phase velocity of optokinetic nystagmus and the visual induction of illusory self-rotation. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J.R., & Graybiel, A., Parabolic flight: Loss of sense orientation. Science, 1979, 206, 1105–1108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Levine, M., Visual direction depends on the operation of spatial constancy mechanisms. Neuroscience Letters, 1978, 7, 207–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Levine, M., Changes in apparent body orientation and sensory localization induced by vibration of postural muscles: Vibratory my esthetic illusions. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1979, 50, 346–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Lobovits, D., Adaptation to displaced vision: Evidence for prolonged aftereffects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1977, 29, 65–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Lobovits, D., Incremental exposure facilitates adaptation to sensory rearrangement. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1978, 49, 362–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J.R., & Teixeira, R., Optokinetic motion sickness: Continuous head movements attenuate the visual induction of apparent self-rotation and symptoms of motion sickness. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1911, 48, 248–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J., & Tuller, B., Role of efference monitoring in the detection of self-produced speech errors. In E.C.T. Walker & W.E. Cooper (Eds.), Sentence Processing. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates, 1979, pp. 281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mach, E., Contributions to the analysis of the sensations. Chicago: Open Court Publishing, 1897.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Magnus, R., Korperstellung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1924.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., & Lackner, J., Adaptation to visual rearrangement elicited by tonic vibration reflexes. Experimental Brain Research, 1975, 24, 103–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., & Lackner, J., Sensory factors in adaptation to visual rearrangement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1911, 29, 237–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., & Lackner, J., Visual tracking of active and passive movements of the hand. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1980, 32, 307–316. (a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., & Lackner, J., Adaptation to visual rearrangement with active and passive limb movements: Effect of movement frequency and predictability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1980, 32, 317–324. (b)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., & Lackner, J., Adaptation to visual displacement: Contribution of proprioceptive, visual, and attentional factors, 1980, in submission, (c)

    Google Scholar 

  • Moulden, B., Adaptation to displaced vision: Reafference is a special case of the cuediscrepancy hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 23, 113–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skowbo, D., Timney, B., Gentry, T., & Morant, R., McCollough effects: Experimental findings and theoretical accounts. Psychological Bulletin, 1975, 82, 497–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbach, M., Eye tracking of self-moved targets: The role of efference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 83, 366–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbach, M., & Held, R., Eye tracking of observer-generated target movements. Science, 1968, 161, 187–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teixeira, R., & Lackner, J., Influence of apparent head position on optokinetic nystagmus and eye posture. Experimental Brain Research, 1976, 24, 435–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teixeira, R., & Lackner, J.R., Optokinetic motion sickness: Attenuation of visually induced illusory self-rotation by passive head movements. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 1979, 50, 264–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Hoist, E., Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 1954, 2, 89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walk, R., Shepherd, J., & Miller, D., Attention as an alternative to self-induced motion for the perceptual behavior of kittens. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 1978, 4, 128, A395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., Informational discrepancy as a basis of perceptual adaptation. In S. Freedman (Ed.), The Neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey Press, 1968, pp. 209 - 229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., On perception. New York: Quadrangle/The New York Times Book Co., 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., & Bacon, J., Two kinds of adaptation in the constancy of visual direction and their different effects on the perception of shape and visual direction. Perception and Psychophysics, 1977, 21, 227 - 242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., & Kravitz, J., The measurement of the constancy of visual direction and of its adaptation. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 2, 217–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, H., & Kravitz, J., Adaptation in the constancy of visual direction tested by measuring the constancy of auditory direction. Perception and Psychophysics, 1968, 4, 299–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R., Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yachzel, B., & Lackner, J., Adaptation to displaced vision: Evidence for transfer of adaptation and long-lasting aftereffects. Perception and Psychophysics, 1977, 22, 147–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1981 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lackner, J.R. (1981). Some Aspects of Sensory-Motor Control and Adaptation in Man. In: Walk, R.D., Pick, H.L. (eds) Intersensory Perception and Sensory Integration. Perception and Perceptual Development. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-9197-9_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-9197-9_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-9199-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-9197-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics