Psychological Time

Memory, Attention, and Consciousness
  • Richard A. Block
  • Dan Zakay

Abstract

All animals must be able to encode temporal properties of events, store representations of those properties, and use those representations for actions. Although no sensory organ or perceptual system uniquely subserves psychological time, temporal properties of events provide important information. More highly evolved animals can also consciously remember the timing of past events and anticipate the timing of future events. They experience several aspects of time, including temporal location, duration, and temporal perspective. Timing of events lasting from seconds to minutes is especially important for survival. Many psychologists have studied duration timing, probably because it is a complex and important aspect in terms of environmental adaptation. Most theorists explain duration timing in terms of cognitive processes, biological processes, or both. Some aspects of duration timing require attention and memory, and the study of duration judgments reveals and clarifies the underlying processes.

Keywords

Dopamine Dock 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Gibson J.J., 1975, Events are perceivable but time is not, The study of time II, Fraser and Lawrence (eds.), Springer-Verlag, New York, 295–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Klemmer E.T., 1957, Simple reaction time as a function of time uncertainty, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54 195–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Michon J.A. and Jackson J.L., 1985, Introduction: The psychology of time, Time, mind, and behavior, Michon and Jackson (eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 2–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Block R.A., 1979, Time and consciousness, Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1. Psychological issues, Underwood and Stevens (eds.), Academic Press, London, 179–217.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Block R.A., 1990, Models of psychological time, Cognitive models of psychological time, Block (ed.), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1–35.Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Block R.A. and Zakay D., 1996, Models of psychological time revisited, Time and mind, Helfrich (ed.), WA: Hogrefe and Huber, Kirkland, 171–195.Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Zakay D. and Block R.A., 1997, Temporal cognition, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6 12–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Hintzman D.L. and Block R.A., 1971, Repetition and memory: Evidence for a multiple-trace hypothesis, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88 297–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Hintzman D.L., Block R.A., and Summers J.J., 1973, Contextual associations and memory for serial position, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97 220–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Hintzman D.L., Block R.A., and Inskeep N.R., 1972, Memory for mode of input, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11 741–749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Hintzman D.L., Block R.A., and Summers J.J., 1973, Modality tags and memory for repetitions: Locus of the spacing effect, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12 229–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Friedman W.J. and Wilkins A.J., 1985, Scale effects in memory for the time of events, Memory & Cognition, 13 168–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Friedman W.J., 1993, Memory for the time of past events, Psychological Bulletin, 113 44–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Shum M.S., 1998, The role of temporal landmarks in autobiographical memory processes, Psychological Bulletin, 124 423–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Auday B.C., Sullivan C., and Cross H.A., 1988, The effects of constrained rehearsal on judgments of temporal order, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26 548–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Clayton N.S. and Dickinson A., 1998, Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays, Nature, 395 272–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Clayton N.S. and Dickinson A., 1999, Memory for the content of caches by scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens), Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25 82–91.Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Fetterman J.G., 1995, The psychophysics of remembered duration, Animal Learning & Behavior, 23 49–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Hintzman D.L., 1970, Effects of repetition and exposure duration on memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83 435–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Block R.A., 1989, Experiencing and remembering time: Affordances, context, and cognition, Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, Levin and Zakay (eds.), North-Holland, Amsterdam, 333–363.Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    James W., 1890, The principles of psychology, Holt, New York, 1 624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Frankenhaeuser M., 1959, Estimation of time: An experimental study, Almqvist and Wiksell, Stockholm.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Hicks R.E., 1992, Prospective and retrospective judgments of time: A neurobehavioral analysis, Time, action and cognition: Towards bridging the gap, Macar, Pouthas, and Friedman (eds.), Netherlands: Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, 97–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Hicks R.E., Miller G.W., and Kinsbourne M., 1976, Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed, American Journal of Psychology, 89 719–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Block R.A. and Zakay D., 1997, Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4 184–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Block R.A., 1992, Prospective and retrospective duration judgment: The role of information processing and memory, Time, action and cognition: Towards bridging the gap, Macar, Pouthas, and Friedman (eds.), Netherlands: Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, 141–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Block R.A., 1982, Temporal judgments and contextual change, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8 530–544.Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Block R.A. and Reed M.A., 1978, Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4 656–665.Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Ornstein R.E., 1969, On the experience of time, Hannondsworth, Penguin, England.Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Fraisse P., 1963 (Original work published 1957), The psychology of time (Leith, Trans.), Harper and Row, NY.Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Poynter W.D., 1983, Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience, Memory & Cognition, 11 77–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Block R.A., 1974, Memory and the experience of duration in retrospect, Memory & Cognition, 2 153–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Poynter W.D., 1989, Judging the duration of time intervals: Aprocess of remembering’ segments of experience, Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective, Levin and Zakay (eds.), North-Holland, Amsterdam, 305–331.Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Block R.A., 1985, Contextual coding in memory: Studies of remembered duration, Time, mind, and behavior, Michon and Jackson (eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 169–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Block R.A., 1986, Remembered duration: Imagery processes and contextual encoding, Acta Psychologica, 62 103–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Wearden J.H. and Ferrara A., 1993, Subjective shortening in humans’ memory for stimulus duration, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 46B 163–186.Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Zakay D., 1993, The roles of non-temporal information processing load and temporal expectations in children’s prospective time estimation, Acta Psychologica, 84 271–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Brown S.W., 1998, Automaticity versus timesharing in timing and tracking dual-task performance, Psychological Research, 61 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Macar F., Grondin S., and Casini L., 1994, Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation, Memory & Cognition, 22 673–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    Thomas E.A.C. and Weaver W.B., 1975, Cognitive processing and time perception, Perception & Psychophysics, 17 363–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Zakay D. and Block R.A., 1996, The role of attention in time estimation processes, Time, internal clocks and movement, Pastor and Artieda (eds.), North-Holland/Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, 143–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Brown S.W., 1997, Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks, Perception & Psychophysics, 59 1118–1140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Zakay D., 1998, Attention allocation policy influences prospective timing, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5 114–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Zakay D., 1992, The role of attention in children’s time perception, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54 355–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Zakay D. and Block R.A., The negative asynchrony phenomenon: A prospective timing perspective, Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events, Aschersleben, Müsseler, and Bachmann (eds.), Elsevier}, AmsterdGoogle Scholar
  46. 46.
    Block R.A., 1996, Psychological time and memory systems of the brain, Dimensions of time and life, Fraser and Soulsby (eds.), CT: International Universities Press, Madison, 61–76.Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Milner B., 1982, Some cognitive effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man, The neuropsychology of cognitive function, Broadbent and Weiskrantz (eds.), The Royal Society, London, 211–226.Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Petrides M. and Milner B., 1982; Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal-and temporal-lobe lesions in man, Neuropsychologia, 20 249–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Milner B., McAndrews M.P., and Leonard G., 1990, Frontal lobes and memory for the temporal order of recent events, Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 55 987–994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. 50.
    Mangels J.A., Gershberg F.B., Shimamura A.P., and Knight R.T., 1996, Impaired retrieval from remote memory in patients with frontal lobe damage, Neuropsychology, 10 32–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Moscovitch M. and Melo B., 1997, Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: Evidence from confabulation and amnesia, Neuropsychologia, 35 1017–1034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Fuster J.M., 1995, Temporal processing, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769 173–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. 53.
    Fuster J.M., 1997, The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe, (3prd ed.), Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  54. 54.
    Moscovitch M., 1992, Memory and working-with-memory: A component process model based on modules and central systems, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4 257–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    McAndrews M.P., and Milner B., 1991, The frontal cortex and memory for temporal order, Neuropsychologia, 29 849–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    Schacter D.L., 1989, Memory, Foundations of cognitive science, Posner (ed.), MIT Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  57. 57.
    Richards W., 1973, Time reproductions by H.M., Acta Psychologica, 37 279–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Williams J.M., Medwedeff C.H., and Haban G., 1989, Memory disorder and subjective time estimation, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11 713–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. 59.
    Goldman-Rakic P.S., Selemon L.D., and Schwartz M.L., 1984, Dual pathways connecting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with the hippocampal formation and parahippocampal cortex in the rhesus monkey, Neuroscience, 12 719–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. 60.
    Olton D.S., 1989, Frontal cortex, timing and memory, Neuropsychologia, 27 121–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Nichelli P., 1993, The neuropsychology of human temporal information processing, Handbook of neuropsychology, Boller and Grafman6 (eds.), 8 339–371.Google Scholar
  62. 62.
    Nichelli P., Clark K., Hollnagel C., and Grafman J., 1995, Duration processing after frontal lobe lesions, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769 183–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. 63.
    Church R.M., 1989, Theories of timing behavior, Contemporary learning theories: Instrumental conditioning theory and the impact of biological constraints on learning, Klein and Mowrer (eds.), NJ: Erlbaum, Hillsdale, 41–71.Google Scholar
  64. 64.
    Rubia K., Overmeyer S., Taylor E., Brammer M., Williams S., Simmons A., Andrew C., and Bullmore E., 1998, Prefrontal involvement in “temporal bridging” and timing movement, Neuropsychologia, 36 1283–1293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Goldman-Rakic P.S., 1992, Working memory and the mind, Scientific American, 267 111–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Sawaguchi T. and Goldman-Rakic P.S., 1991, D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: Involvement in working memory, Science, 251 947–950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Meck W.H., 1996, Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception, Cognitive Brain Research, 3 227–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Spiegel E.A., Wycis H.T., Orchinik C.W., and Freed H., 1955, The thalamus and temporal orientation, Science, 121 771–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Spiegel E.A., Wycis H.T., Orchinik C., and Freed H., 1956, Thalamic chronotaraxis, American Journal of Psychiatry, 113 97–105.Google Scholar
  70. 70.
    Spiegel E.A. and Wycis H.T., 1967, Multiple representation of various functions in the human subcortex, Confinia Neurologica, 29 163–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. 71.
    Spiegel E.A. and Wycis H.T., 1968, Multiplicity of subcortical localization of various functions, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 147 45–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Markowitsch H.J., 1992, Intellectualfunctions of the brain: An historical perspective, Hogrefe and Huber,Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
  73. 73.
    Markowitsch H.J., 1995, Cerebral bases of consciousness: A historical view, Neuropsychologia, 33 1181–1192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Payk T.R., 1977, Störungen des Zeiterlebens bei den endogenen Psychosen [Disturbances of time experience in endogenous psychoses], Schweizer Archiv für Neurologie, Neurochirurgie und Psychiatrie, 121 277–285.Google Scholar
  75. 75.
    Binkofski F. and Block R.A., 1996, Accelerated time after left frontal cortex lesion, Neurocase, 2 485–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Posner M.I. and Raichle M.E., 1994, Images of mind, Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  77. 77.
    Kesner R.P., 1998, Neural mediation of memory for time: Role of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5 585–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic / Plenum Publishers, New York 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Richard A. Block
    • 1
  • Dan Zakay
    • 2
  1. 1.Montana State UniversityBozemanUSA
  2. 2.Tel Aviv UniversityTel AvivIsrael

Personalised recommendations