Skip to main content

Social Judgment as Implicit Social Comparison

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: The Springer Series in Social Clinical Psychology ((SSSC))

Abstract

At first blush, this chapter will appear to be in the wrong book. The volume focuses on what theorists and researchers know and sometimes suspect about social comparison processes. By definition, these are processes by which people come to know and understand themselves through comparing themselves and their behavior with other people. Over the years, researchers have discovered quite a bit about what spurs people on to make social comparisons, the processes by which they make those comparisons, the people that they choose to compare themselves with, and the consequences of this comparison activity (for reviews beyond this volume, see Collins, 1996; Suls & Wills, 1991; Wills, 1981; Wood, 1989). These researchers have discovered that social comparison is a frequent and ubiquitous activity in the mental life of the social animal, with consequences for how people think about themselves and their social worlds. Indeed, these discoveries began with Leon Festinger’s classic article on the topic in 1954, and the decades have diminished neither the importance nor the value of theories of social comparison.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ajzen, I. (1977). Intuitive theories of events and the effects of base-rate information on prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621–1630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alicke, M. D. (1993). Egocentric standards of conduct evaluation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 14, 171–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alicke, M. D., LoSchiavo, F. M., Zerbst, J., & Zhang, S. (1997). The person who outperforms me is a genius: Maintaining perceived competence in upward social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 781–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. E. (1902). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauregard, K. S., & Dunning, D. (1998). Turning up the contrast: Self-enhancement motives prompt egocentric contrast effects in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 606–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. D. (1986). Evaluations of self and others: Self-enhancement biases in social judgment. Social Cognition, 4, 353–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruvold, W. H. (1975). Judgmental bias in the rating of attitude statements. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 35, 605–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. D. (1986). Similarity and uniqueness: The effects of attribute type, relevance, and individual differences in self-esteem and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 281–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. L. (1988). Self-relevance and goal-directed processing in the recall of weighting of information about others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 310–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catambone, R., & Markus, H. (1987). The role of self-schemas in going beyond the information given. Social Cognition, 5, 349–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social comparison on self-evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Combe, A. W., & Snygg, D. (1949). Individual behavior: A perceptual approach to behavior. New York: Harper & Brothers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corenblum, B., & Corfield, V. K. (1976). The effect of attitude and statement of favorability upon the judgment of attitude statements. Social Behavior and Personality, 4, 249–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawes, R. M. (1989). Statistical criteria for a truly false consensus effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dornbusch, S. M., Hastorf, A. J., Richardson, S. A., Muzzy, R. E., & Vreeland, R. S. (1965). The perceiver and the perceived: Their relative influence on the categories of interpersonal cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D. (1998). Linking self to social judgment: On the symbolic management of social judgment. Unpublished manuscript. Cornell University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D. (1999). A newer look: Motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D. (in press). On the motives underlying social cognition. In N. Schwarz & A. Tesser (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Volume 1: Intrapersonal processes. New York: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Beauregard, K. S. (in press). Regulating impressions of others to affirm images of the self. Social Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Cohen, G. L. (1992). Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 341–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Griffin, D. W., Milojkovic, J. H., & Ross, L. (1990). The overconfidence effect in social prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 568–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Hayes, A. F. (1996). Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 213–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Leuenberger, A., & Sherman, D. A. (1995). A new look at motivated inference: Are self-serving theories of success a product of motivational forces? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 58–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & McElwee, R. O. (1995). Idiosyncratic trait definitions: Implications for self-description and social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 936–946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082–1090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Perie, M., & Story, A. L. (1991). Self-serving prototypes of social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 957–968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eiser, J. R., & Mower White, C. J. (1974). Evaluative consistency and social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 349–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eiser, J. R., & Mower White, C. J. (1975). Categorization and congruity in attitudinal judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 769–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1924/1956). Further remarks on the defense neuropsychoses. In Collected papers of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 1, pp. 155–182). London: Hogarth Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 227–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruder, C. L. (1977). Choice of comparison persons in evaluating oneself. In J. M. Suls & R. C. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 21–42). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackmiller, K. L. (1966). Threat as a determinant of downward comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(Suppl. 1), 32–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. S. (1898). Some aspects of the early sense of self. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 351–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Dunning, D. (1997). Construal processes and trait ambiguity: Implications for self-peer agreement in personality judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 664–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helgeson, V. S., & Mickelson, K. D. (1995). Motives for social comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1200–1209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Lurie, L. (1983). Context, categorization, and memory: The “change-of-standard” effect. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 525–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinckley, E., & Rethlingshafer, D. (1951). Value judgments of heights of men by college students. Journal of Psychology, 31, 257–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C. I., Harvey, O. J., & Sherif, M. (1957). Assimilation and contrast effects in reactions to communications and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 244–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hovland, C. I., & Sherif, M. (1952). Judgmental phenomena and scales of attitude measurement: Item displacement in Thurstone scales. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 822–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1915). Psychology: Briefer course. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Gerard, H. B. (1967). Foundations of social psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., Rock, L., Shaver, K. G., Goethals, G. R., & Ward, L. M. (1968). Pattern of performance and ability attribution: An unexpected primacy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 317–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M. (1979). Consensus information, prediction, and causal attribution: A review of the literature and issues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1966–1981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krech, D., & Crutchfield, R. S. (1948). Theory and problems of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. (1998). On the perception of social consensus. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 163–240). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Clement, R. W. (1994). The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 596–610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Clement, R. W. (1997). Consensus estimates by majorities and minorities: The case for social projection. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 299–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A. J., & Wedell, D. H. (1991). The self and social judgment: Effects of affective reaction and “own position” on judgments of unambiguous and ambiguous information about others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 884–898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemon, N., & Warren, N. (1974). Salience, centrality, and self-relevance of traits in construing others. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 119–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P. (1983). Self-image bias in person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 384–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewicki, P. (1984). Self-schema and social information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1177–1190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, E. (1943). Skin color judgments of Negro college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 388, 370–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, G. (1984). Thinking one’s abilities are unique and one’s opinions are common. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 203–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, G., & Miller, N. (1987). Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: An empirical and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 72–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Smith, J. (1981). The influence of self-schemas on the perception of others. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition, and social interaction (pp. 233–262). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., Smith, J., & Moreland, R. L. (1985). Role of the self-concept in the social perception of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1494–1512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, L. Z. (1976). The lesser influence of consensus than distinctiveness information on causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 733–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, W. (1921). An introduction to social psychology. Boston: Luce.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettee, D. R., & Smith, G. (1977). Social comparison and interpersonal attraction: the case for dissimilarity. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 69–101). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. T. (1984). Self-schemas, gender, and social comparison: A clarification of the related attributes hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1222–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, B., & Goethals, G. R. (1990). Social projection, actual consensus and valence. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 279–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. (1947). Personality: A biosocial approach to origin and structure. New York: Harper.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Borgida, E. (1975). Attribution and the psychology of prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 932–943.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, B. W., & Wachsmuth, J. O. (1995). The waxing and waning of the social self: Assimilation and contrast in social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 825–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pittman, T. S. (1998). Motivation. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 549–590). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. (1990). Familiarity and differences in self-and other-representation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 369–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prothro, E. T. (1955). The effect of strong negative affect on the placement of items in a Thurstone scale. Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 11–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N., & Olson, J. (1995). What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. P. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romer, D. (1983). Effects of own attitude on polarization of judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 273–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation and contrast effects in communication and attitude change. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M., & Jackman, N. (1966). Judgments of truth in collective controversy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 30, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. H., Hilton, D. J., Kim, S. H., & Garonzik, R. (1992). Knowledge-based causal inference: Norms and the usefulness of distinctiveness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 239–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, H. S. (1947). Conceptions of modern psychiatry. Washington, DC: William Alanson White Psychiatry Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suls, J., & Wills, T. A. (1991). Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1278–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A., & Campbell, J. (1983). Self-definition and self-evaluation maintenance. In J. Suls & A. Greenwald (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 1–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Upshaw, H. S. (1975). The effect of variable perspectives on judgments of opinion statements for Thurstone scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 60–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallone, R. P., Griffin, D. W., Lin, S., & Ross, L. (1990). Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 582–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 806–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Harvey, J. H. (1977). Do people use consensus information in making causal attributions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, L. (1966). Motivation as a determinant of upward comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 (Suppl. 1), 27-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, L., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). The proxy model of social comparison for self-assessment of ability. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 54–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windschitl, P. D., & Wills, G. L. (1997). Behavioral consensus information affects people’s inferences about population traits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 148–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparison of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V (1996). What is social comparison and how should we study it? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 520–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanna, M. P., Goethals, G. R., & Hill, J. F. (1975). Evaluating a sex-related ability: Social comparison with similar others and standard setters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 86–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2000 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dunning, D. (2000). Social Judgment as Implicit Social Comparison. In: Suls, J., Wheeler, L. (eds) Handbook of Social Comparison. The Springer Series in Social Clinical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-4237-7_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-4237-7_17

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-6903-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-4237-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics