Derivational Morphology in Sli Children

Structure and Semantics of Hebrew Nouns
  • Dorit Ravid
  • Galit Avivi
  • Ronit Levy

Abstract

The domain of derivational morphology has not been studied extensively in the language of SLI children. This paper examines comprehension and production of Hebrew nouns in language-impaired compared with normally developing schoolchildren with a view to contribute to the theoretical discussion of the nature of childhood SLI.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Berman, R.A. (1985). Acquisition of Hebrew. In D.I. Slobin (ed.) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol I. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 255–371.Google Scholar
  2. Berman, R.A. (1987a). Productivity in the lexicon: new-word formation in Modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica 21, 225–254.Google Scholar
  3. Berman, R.A. (1987b). A developmental route: leaning about the form and use of complex nominals in Hebrew. Linguistics 25, 1057–1085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berman, R.A. (1993). Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 20, 641–669.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berman, R.A. (In press). From known to new: How children coin nouns compared with verbs in Hebrew. In L. Menn, & N. Bernstein-Ratner (Eds.) Festschrift for Jean Berko Gleason. Google Scholar
  6. Carlisle, J.F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics 9, 247–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of developmental dysphasia. Linguistics 27, 897–920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clark, E.V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clark, E.V., & R.A. Berman. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word formation. Language 60, 542–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. de Weck, G. (1998). Anaphoric cohesion in young language-impaired and normally developing children. In A. Aksu-Koç, E. Erguvanli Taylan, A. Sumru Özsoy, & A. Küntay (Eds.) Perspectives on language acquisition: Selected papers from the VIIth International Congress for the Study of Child Language (pp. 292–308). Istanbul: Bogaziçi University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Dromi, E., L.B. Leonard, & M. Shteiman. (1993). The grammatical morphology of Hebrew-speaking children with specific language impairment: Some competing hypotheses. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 760--771.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Freyd, E, & J. Baron. (1982). Individual differences in acquisition of derivational morphology. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 21, 282–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Johnston, J.R. (1994). Cognitive abilities of children with language impairment. in R.V. Watkins, & M.L. Rice (Eds.) Specific language impairments in children (pp. 107–121). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  14. Leonard, L.B. (1994). Some problems facing accounts of morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment. in R.V. Watkins, & M.L. Rice (Eds.) Specific language impairments in children (pp. 91–105). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  15. Leonard, L.B., V. Bortolini, M.C. Caselli, K.K. McGregor, & L. Sabbadini. (1992). Morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment: the status of features in the underlying grammar. Language Acquisition 2, 151–179.Google Scholar
  16. Miller, J.F., & T. Klee. (1995). Phonological impairment. In P. Fletcher, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.) The handbook of child language (pp. 573–602). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  17. Moats, L.C., & C. Smith. (1992). Derivational morphology: why it should be included in language assessment and instruction. Language,Speech cinc! Hearing Services in Schools 23, 312–319.Google Scholar
  18. Nagy, W.E., R.C. Anderson, M. Scommer, J.A. Scott, & A.C. Stellmen. (1989). Reading Research Quautely 24, 262–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nippold, M.A. (in press). Defining abstract entities: development in pre-adolescents, adolescents, and young adults. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Reseach. Google Scholar
  20. Paul, R. (1995). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Assessment and intervention. St. Louis: Mosby.Google Scholar
  21. Ravid, D. (1990). Internal structure constraints on new-word formation devices in modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica 24, 289–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ravid, D. (To appear). The development of Hebrew orthography in gradeschool children. In E. Olshtayn, S. Blum-Kolka, & O.R. Schwarzwald (Eds.) Festschrift for R. Nie Jerusalem: Hebrew University.Google Scholar
  23. Ravid, D., & A. Avidor. (1998). Acquisition of derived nominals in Hebrew: developmental and linguistic prin-ciples. Journal of Child Language 25, 229–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ravid, D., & A. Malenky. (Submitted). The development of morphological awareness in Hebrew: The route to explicitation.Google Scholar
  25. Rice, M.L. (1994). Grammatical categories of children with specific language impairments. In R.V. Watkins, &M.L. Rice (Eds.) Specific language impairments in children (pp. 69–89).Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  26. Rom, A., & L.B. Leonard. (1990). Interpreting deficits in grammatical morphology in specifically language-impaired children: Preliminary evidence from Hebrew. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 4, 93–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing ability. Language and Speech 31, 337–355.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Rubin, H., M. Kantor, & J. Macnab. (1990). Grammatical awareness in the spoken and written language of language-disabled children. Canadian Journal of Psychology 44, 483–500.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schery, T.K. (1985). Correlates of language development in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 50, 73–83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Stark, R.F., & P. Tallal. (1981). Selection of children with specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46, 114–122.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Stone, C.A., & P.J. Connell. (1993). Induction of a visual symbolic rule in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 599–608.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Swisher, L., & D. Snow. (1994). Learning and generalization components of morphological acquisition by children with SLI: Is there a functional relation? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 1406–1413.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Swisher, L., M.A. Restrepo, E. Plante, & S. Lowell. (1995). Effect of implicit and explicit “rule” presentation on bound-morpheme generalization in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 168–173.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Weiss, A.L. (1997). Planning language intervention for young children. In D.K. Bernstein, & E. Tiegerman-Farber (Eds.) (pp. 272–323) Language and communication disorders in children. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dorit Ravid
    • 1
    • 2
  • Galit Avivi
    • 2
  • Ronit Levy
    • 2
  1. 1.School of EducationUSA
  2. 2.The Department of Communication DisordersTel Aviv UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations