Skip to main content

Relations Between Method and Theory in Developmental Research: A partial-isomorphism view

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Annals of Theoretical Psychology ((AOTP,volume 7))

Summary

This chapter argues that the linkage in developmental psychology between methods and theory (conceived of as broadly defined viewpoints and frameworks) is one of ‘partial-isomorphism’, that is, of imperfect correlation. The chapter begins with a general examination and critique of current views of the issue of the relation between theory and method, introducing two specific examples, one from an earlier period of the history of developmental psychology, the other from more current work in the field. The relationship between theory and method is then analyzed more specifically, with reference to two prototypical areas of developmental concern: the stage issue, and the continuity/stability issue. It is argued that a relationship of ‘partial isomorphism’ not only does fullest justice to the way in which developmental investigators conduct their research, but also that this construal of the theory-method relation is most apt to advance scientific progress in our field. The flexible, loose sort of linkage between theory and method stressed in this chapter will serve as a counterforce to sterile pursuit of methodology for its own sake, divorced from and uninformed by theory, such as would be encouraged if methodology were to be considered as completely independent of theory. At the same time the conception likewise avoids the excesses of theorizing without regard to methodological approach, or of subordinating method entirely to theory, which is apt to ensure the preservation of the theory in isolation from rival ones, and thus lead eventually to its dying on the vine. The final section of the paper is devoted to some specific methodological issues arising in developmental research that are implied in a concept of development, though they are to a considerable extent independent of particular theoretical positions concerning developmental processes.

The writer wishes to acknowledge the helpful comments on portions of this paper offered by Richard M. Loner and John R. Nesselroade.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ainsworth, M. S. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In B. Caldwell & H. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 3, pp. 1–94). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1933). Mental growth during the first three years: A developmental study of sixty-one children by repeated tests. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 14, 1–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilin, H. (1971). Developmental stages and developmental processes. In D. R. Green (Ed.), Measurement and Piaget (pp. 172–189). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. Q., & Harper, R. V. (1974). Child effects on adults. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1978). The interdependence of theory, methodology, and empirical data: Causal modeling as an approach to construct validation. In D. B. Kandel (Ed.), Longitudinal research on drug use: Empirical findings and methodological issues (pp. 267–302). Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C. (1963). Some persisting dilemmas in the measurement of change. In C. W. Harris (Ed.), Problems in measuring change (pp. 3–20). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics. Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1961). Child development. Vol. 1:A systematic and empirical theory. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1963). Some methodological contributions from a functional analysis of child behavior. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. C. Spiker (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 1 (pp. 197–231). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. (1964). Stability and change in human characteristics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1974). Training and transfer of transitivity, conservation, and class inclusion. Child Development, 48, 360–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1977). Response criteria in concept development research. Child Development, 38, 360–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 173–182; 207–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Matter, J., & Papanek, M. L. (1955). Breadth of learning as a function of drive level and mechanization. Psychological Review, 62, 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. L. (1958). The inheritance of mental ability. American Psychologist, 13, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1963). The structure of change by P-technique and incremental R-technique. In C. W. Harris (Ed.), Problems of measuring change (pp. 167–198). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J., & Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral judgment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48,(1–2) (Whole No. 200).

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, W. A. (Ed.). (1982). The concept of development. (Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology), Vol. 15. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connell, J. P., & Tanaka, J. S. (Eds.). (1987). Special section on structural equation modeling. Child Development, 58, 1–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crutchfield, R. S., & Krech, D. (1961). Some guides to the understanding of the history of psychology. In L. Postman (Ed.), Psychology in the making (pp. 3–30). New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmerich, W. (1964). Continuity and stability in early social development. Child Development, 35, 311–332.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle: Selected papers. Psychological Issues, 1(1) (Whole No. 1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1961). Learning theory and the new ‘mental chemistry.’ Psychological Review, 67, 207–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1983). Commentary. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48,(1–2) (Whole No. 200).

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1972). An analysis of cognitive-developmental sequences. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 86, 279–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1982). Structures, stages, and sequences in cognitive development. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), The concept of development. Minnesota Symposium on Child Development, Vol. 15 (pp. 1–28). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Wohlwill, J. F. (1969). Formal and functional aspects of cognitive development. In D. Elkind & J. H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development (pp. 67–120). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1968). Psychological explanation: An introduction to the philosophy of psychology. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1985). Mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L., & Amatruda, C. S. (1941). Developmental diagnosis: normal and abnormal child development. New York: Hoeber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gewirtz, J. (1972). Attachment, dependence, and a distinction in terms of stimulus control. In J. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (pp. 139–177). Washington, D.C.: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldschmid, M. L., & Bentler, P. M. (1968). The dimensions and measurement of conservation. Child Development, 39, 787–802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. W. (1984). Home environment and early cognitive development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. W. (1963). Problems in measuring change. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, D. H. (1957). The concept of development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1987). Beyond autoregressive models: Some implications of the trait-state distinction for the structural modeling of developmental change. Child Development, 58, 93–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, K., & McHale, S. (1987). Sources of stability and change in early childhood. In D. Peters & S. Kontos (Eds.), Annual Advances in Applied Developmental Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 17–39). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooper, F. H., Toniolo, T. A., & Sipple, T. S. (1978). A longitudinal analysis of logical reasoning relationships: conservation and transitive inferen-cer. Developmental Psychology, 14, 674–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and experience. New York: Ronald.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. McV. (1979). Psychological development: Early experience. Annual Review of Psychology, 30, 103–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutt, C., & Bhavnani, R. (1972). Prediction from play. Nature, 237, 171–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1978). On the ontogenesis of emotions and emotion-cognition relationships in infancy. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The origins of affect (pp. 389–413). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K. (1979). Statistical estimation of structural models in longitudinal-developmental investigations. In J. R. Nesselroade & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development (pp. 303–352). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1971). Change and continuity in infancy. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1976). Resilience and continuity in psychological development. In A. M. Clarke & A. D. B. Clarke (Eds.), Early experience: myth and evidence (pp. 97–121). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1980). Perspectives on continuity. In O. Brim & J. Kagan (Eds.), Constacy and change in human development (pp. 26–74). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, T. S. (1979). The development of discrimination-learning: A levels of functioning explanation. In H. W. Reese & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 83–177). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, T. S., & Kendler, H. (1970). An ontogeny of optional shift behavior. Child Development, 41, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kessel, F. (1969). The philosophy of science as proclaimed and science as practiced: Identity or dualism? American Psychologist, 24, 999–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). The meaning and measurement of moral development. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. (Heinz-Werner Lecture Series, Vol. 15).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development. Vol. 2: The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuenne, M. R. (1946). Experimental investigation of the relation of language to transposition behavior in young children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 471–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1972). Mechanisms of change in the development of cognitive structures. Child Development, 43, 833–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1970). Werner’s comparative organismic theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology, (3rd ed). (Vol. 1, pp. 733–771). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leiter, R. G. (1940). The Leiter International Performance Scale. Santa Barbara, CA: Santa Barbara State College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1986). Concepts and theories of human development (2nd ed.). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (Eds.). (1981). Individuals as producers of their own development: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S. (1987). Information processing and Piagetian theory: Conflict or congruence? In L. S. Liben, (Ed.), Development and learning: Conflict or congruence? (pp. 109–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. A. (1981). A longitudinal study of the consequences of early mother-infant interaction: A microanalytic approach. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(3), (Whole No. 190).

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1946). Problem-centering vs. means-centering in science. Philosophy of Science, 13, 326–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J. (1988a). Dynamic but structural equation modeling of repeated measures data. In J. R. Nesselroade & R. B. Cattell (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology (Vol 2, pp. 561–614). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J., & Epstein, D. (1987). Latent growth curves within developmental structural equation models. Child Development, 58, 110–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCall, R. B. (1979). The development of intellectual functioning in infancy and the prediction of later IQ. In J. D. Osofsky, (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 707–741). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, R. B. (1981). Nature and nurture and the two realms of development: A proposed integration with respect to mental development. Child Development, 52, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, M. B. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. J., Cohen, L. B., & Hill, K. T. (1970). A methodological investigation of Piaget’s theory of object concept development in the sensory-motor period. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9, 59–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moshman, D. (1977). Consolidation of stage formation in the emergence of formal operations. Developmental Psychology, 13, 95–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muchow, M., & Muchow, H. (1935). Der Lebensraum des Grosstadtkindes. Hamburg: Reigel (reprinted Bensheim, Germany: Päd-extra, 1978).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nassefat, M. (1963). Etude quantitative sur l’éolution des opérations intellectuelles. Neuchatel: Delachaux & Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R. (1970). Application of multivariate strategies to problems of measuring and structuring long-term change. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology (pp. 193–207). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1979). Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R., & Ford, D. H. (1985). P-technique comes of age: Multivariate, replicated, single-subject designs for research on older subjects. Research on aging. 7, 46–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1957). A behaviorist analysis of perception and meaning as cognitive phenomena. In J. S. Bruner (Ed.), Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 75–118). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F. (1976). The active organism in structuralism. Human Development, 19, 71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F., & Reese, H. W. (1973). Models of development: Methodological implications. In J. R. Nesselroade & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues (pp. 65–86). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paraskevopoulos, J., & Hunt, J. McV. (1971). Object construction and imitation under different conditions of rearing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 119, 301–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1980). Constructive problems for constructive theories: The current relevance of Piaget’s work and a critique of information-processing simulation psychology. In R. H. Kluwe, & H. Spada (Eds.), Developmental models of thinking (pp. 263–296). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R., Littman, R. A., & Bricker, W. (1967). Assertive behavior in children: A step toward a theory of aggression. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 32,(5) (Whole No. 113).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepper, S. C. (1942). World hypotheses: A study in evidence. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1969). The mechanisms of perception. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Lambercier, M. (1946). Recherches sur le développement des perceptions: VIII. Transpositions perceptives et transitivité opératoire dans les comparaisons en profondeur. Archives de Psychologie, Genéve, 31, 325–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Morf, A. (1958). Les isomorphismes partiels entre les structures logiques at les structures perceptives. In J. Piaget (Ed.), Etudes d’epistémologie génétique (Vol. 6, pp. 49–116). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Taponier, S. (1956). Recherches sur le développement des perceptions: XXXII. Eestimation des longeurs de deux droites horizontales et paralleles à extremitées décalées. Archives de Psychologie, Genéve, 35, 369–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinard, A., & Laurendeau, M. (1969). ’stage’ in Piaget’s cognitive-developmental theory: Exegesis of a concept. In D. Elkind & J. H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development (pp. 121–163). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., & Foch, T. T. (1980). A twin study of objectively assessed personality in childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 680–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1970). The scope of experimental child psychology. In H. W. Reese and L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Experimental child psychology (pp. 1–12). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 115–145). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J. (1975). Early influences on development. Merill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 267–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype — environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W. (Ed.). (1984). Longitudinal studies of adult psychological development. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoggen, P. (1985). Martha Muchow: Precursor to ecological psychology. Human Development, 228, 213–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulsinger, F., Mednick, S. A., & Knop, J. (Eds.). (1981). Longitudinal research: Methods and uses in behavioral science. Boston: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shirley, M. M. (1933). The first two years. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1983). Information-processing approaches to development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed.) (Vol. 1, pp. 129–212). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (1983). Development of concrete transitivity of length in children. Child Development, 34, 389–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, A. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 462–518). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S. (Ed.). (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabor, L. E., & Kendler, T. S. (1981). Testing for developmental continuity or discontinuity: Class inclusion and reversal shifts. Developmental Review, 1, 330–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, W. R., & Grusec, J. (1970). Studies of early experience. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed.). (Vol. 1, pp. 565–654). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Eisert, D. C., Kahle, L. R., Hardy-Brown, K., & Keasey, B. (1979). Child Development, 50, 1153–1163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, L. R. (1963). Implications of factor-analysis of three-way matrices for measurement of change. In C. W. Harris (Ed.), Problems of measuring change (pp. 122–137). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, L. R. (1966). Learning theory and multivariate experiment: Illustrations by determination of generalized learning curves. In R. B. Cattell (Ed.), Handbook of multivariate experimental research (pp. 476–501). Chicago: Rand-McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. (1966). An experimental test of the sequentiality of the child’s moral judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 611–618.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. McV. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Kindermann, T. (1986, September). Everyday reality of canalyzed child development: Qualitative and quantitative aspects of interdependence of theory and empirical research. Paper presented at 2nd European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Rome.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vondracek, F. W., Lerner, R. M., & Schulenberg, J. E. (1986). Career development: A life-span developmental approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D. (1976). Utilization of a Piagetian approach in the investigation of early experience effects: A research strategy and some illustrative data. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 22, 11–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, S. (1985). Martha Muchow and organismic-developmental theory. Human Development, 28, 209–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1957a). Comparative psychology of mental development (Revised ed.). New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1957b). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. H. (1970). The learning theory tradition and child psychology. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed.) (Vol. 1, pp. 657–702). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J.F. (1973). The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1980). [Stability and change of] cognitive development in childhood. In O. G. Brim, Jr. & J. Kagan (Eds.), Constancy and change in human development (pp. 359–444). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1983). Physical and social environment as factors in development. In D. Magnusson & V. L. Allen (Eds.), Human development: An interactional perspective (pp. 111–129). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1985). Martha Muchow and the life-space of the urban child. Human Development, 28, 200–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. E, Fusaro, L., & Devoe, S. (1969, March). Measurement, sedation, and conservation: A longitudinal examination of their interrelationship. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Santa Monica, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Zigler, E. (1963). Metatheoretical issues in developmental psychology. In M. Marx (Ed.), Theories in contemporary psychology (pp. 341–369). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wohlwill, J.F. (1991). Relations Between Method and Theory in Developmental Research: A partial-isomorphism view. In: Van Geert, P., Mos, L.P. (eds) Annals of Theoretical Psychology. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, vol 7. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-3842-4_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-3842-4_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-6714-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-3842-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics