Advertisement

Variability in Cone Populations and Implications

  • Joel Pokorny
  • Vivianne C. Smith
  • Michael F. Wesner
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (NSSA, volume 203)

Abstract

There have been a number of techniques directed toward estimating the relative numbers of the three cone types in the retinae of living humans. For the S-cones, psychophysical measurements which are dependent on the spatial distribution of S-cones (Stiles, 1949; Wald, 1967; Williams et al., 1981) give data which are consistent with the relative sparcity observed anatomically (Ahnelt et al., 1987; deMonasterio et al., 1985; Marc and Sperling, 1977). For typical color-normal observers, the L-and M-cones are both in relatively high spatial density, and the direct psychophysical techniques which are effective for characterizing the retinal distribution of S-cones do not yield interpretable information (Brindley, 1954; Green, 1968; Kelly, 1974). Additional techniques in the literature which have been suggested as capable of revealing L/M cone ratios include Weber fractions for mechanisms isolated by chromatic adaptation (Vos and Walraven, 1971) and retinal densitometry with chromatic bleaching lights (Rushton and Baker, 1964). In this paper we wish to describe our work on two techniques we have used to estimate the L/M cone ratio, spectral sensitivity from heterochromatic flicker photometry and point source detection for lights of varying wavelength. We then consider the perceptual consequences of individual variation in L/M cone populations.

Keywords

Vision Research Spectral Sensitivity Color Vision Psychometric Function Optical Society 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adam, A. (1969). Foveal red-green rataca cf normals, colorblinds and heterozygotes. Proceedings Tel-Hashomer Hospital (Tel-Aviv). 8: 2–6.Google Scholar
  2. Ahnelt, P., H. Kolb and R. Pflug. (1987). Identification of a subtype of cone photoreceptor, likely to be blue sensitive, in the human retina. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 255: 18–34.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alpern, M. and E. Pugh. (1977). Variation in the action spectrum of erythrolabe among deuteranopes. Journal of Physiology (London). 266: 613–646.Google Scholar
  4. Boynton, R. (1979). Human Color Vision. New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  5. Brindley, G. (1954). The summation areas of human colour-receptive mechanisms at increment threshold. Journal of Physiology (London). 124: 400–408.Google Scholar
  6. Burns, S. and A. Elsner. (1985). Color matching at high illuminances: the color-match-area-effect and photopigment bleaching. Journal of the Optical Society of America A,. 2: 698–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cicerone, C. (1987). Constraints placed on color vision models by the relative numbers of different cone classes in human fovea centralis. Die Farbe. 34: 59–66.Google Scholar
  8. Cicerone, C. and J. Nerger. (1989a). The relative numbers of longwavelength-sensitive to middle-wavelength-sensitive cones in the human fovea centralis. Vision Research. 26: 115–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cicerone, C. and J. Nerger. (1989b). The density of cones in the fovea centralis of the human dichromat. Vision Research. 26: 1587–1595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Crone, R. (1959). Spectral sensitivity in color-defective subjects and heterozygous carriers. American Journal of Ophthalmology. 48: 231–238PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. deMonasterio, F., E. McCrane, J. Newlander and S. Schein. (1985). Density profile of blue-sensitive cones along the horizontal meridian of macaque retina. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science. 26: 289–302.Google Scholar
  12. deVries, H. L. (1947). The heredity of the relative numbers of red and green receptors in the human eye. Genetica. 24: 199–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dimmick, F. and M. Hubbard. (1939). The spectral location of psychologically unique yellow, green, and blue. American Journal of Psychology. 52: 242–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eisner, A. and D. MacLeod. (1981). Flicker photometric study of chromatic adaptation: selective suppression of cone inputs by colored backgrounds. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 71: 705–718.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Green, D. (1968). The contrast sensitivity of the colour mechanisms of the human eye. Journal of Physiology (London). 196: 415–429.Google Scholar
  16. Helve, J. (1972). A comparative study of several diagnostic tests of colour vision used for measuring types and degrees of congenital red-green defects. Acta Ophthalmologica Supplement. 115: 1–64.Google Scholar
  17. Hurvich, L. and D. Jameson. (1955). Some quantitative aspects of an opponent-colors theory. II. Brightness, saturation and hue in normal and dichromatic vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 45: 602–616.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hurvich, L. and D. Jameson. (1964). Does anomalous color vision imply color weakness? Psychonomic Science. 1: 11–12Google Scholar
  19. Ingling, C. and B. Tsou. (1977). Orthogonal combination of three visual channels. Vision Research. 17: 1075–1082.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Judd, D. (1951). Colorimetry and artificial daylight, in Technical Committee No. 7 Report of Secretariat United States Commission, International Commission on Illumination, Twelfth Session, Stockholm, pp 1–60.Google Scholar
  21. Kelly, D. (1974). Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of color-vision mechanisms. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 64: 983–990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lutze, M., N. Cox, V. Smith and J. F. J. Smith (1990). Genetic studies of variation in Rayleigh and photometric matches in normal trichromats. Vision Research. 30: 149–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Marc, R. and H. Sperling. (1977). Chromatic organization of primate cones. Science. 143: 454–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Marré, M. and E. Marré. (1984). Rayleigh equation in acquired color vision defects. Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series. 39: 165–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Neitz, J. and G. Jacobs. (1986). Polymorphism of the long-wavelength cone in normal human colour vision. Nature. 323: 623–625.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pokorny, J. and V. Smith. (1977). Evaluation of single pigment shift model of anomalous trichromacy. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 67: 1196–1209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pokorny, J. and V. Smith. (1987). L/M cone ratios and the null point of the perceptual red/green opponent system. Die Farbe. 34: 53–57Google Scholar
  28. Pokorny, J., V. Smith and M. Lutze. (1987). Aging of the human lens.Applied Optics. 26: 1437–1440.Google Scholar
  29. Pokorny, J., V. Smith and M. Lutze. (1988). Sources of shared variance in Rayleigh and photometric matches. OSA Annual Meeting, 1988 Technical Digest Series, Washington, DC. 11: 64.Google Scholar
  30. Rubin, M. (1961). Spectral hue loci of normal and anomalous trichromates. American Journal of Ophthalmology. 52: 166–172.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Rushton, W. and H. Baker. (1964). Red/green sensitivity in normal vision. Vision Research. 4: 75–85.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Schmidt, I. (1955). Some problems related to testing color vision with the Nagel anomaloscope. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 45: 514–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Smith, V. and J. Pokorny. (1975). Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm. Vision Research. 15: 161–171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Smith, V., J. Pokorny and S. Starr. (1976). Variability of color mixture data - I. Interobserver variability in the unit coordinates. Vision Research. 16: 1087–1094.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stiles, W. (1949). Increment thresholds and the mechanisms of colour vision. Documenta Ophthalmologica. 3: 138–163.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Vimal, R., J. Pokorny, V. Smith and S. Shevell. (1989). Foveal cone thresholds. Vision Research. 29: 61–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Vos, J. and P. Walraven. (1971). On the derivation of the foveal receptor primaries. Vision Research. 11: 799–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wald, G. (1967). Blue-blindness in the human fovea. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 57: 1289–1301.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wallatein, R. (1981). Photopigment variation and the perception of equilibrium yellow. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
  40. Webster, M. and D. MacLeod. (1988). Factors underlying individual differences in the color matches of normal observers. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 5: 1722–1735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wesner, M., J. Pokorny, S. Shevell and V. Smith. (in press). Foveal cone detection statistics in color-normals and dichromats. Vision Research. in press Google Scholar
  42. Werner, M., J. Pokorny, V. Smith and D. Feige. (1988). Effect of detection criterion on foveal cone thresholds. OSA Annual Meeting, 1988 Technical Digest Series, Washington, DC. 11: 67.Google Scholar
  43. Williams, D., D. MacLeod and M. Hayhoe. (1981). Punctate sensitivity of the blue-sensitive mechanism. Vision Research. 21: 1357–1375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joel Pokorny
    • 1
  • Vivianne C. Smith
    • 1
  • Michael F. Wesner
    • 1
  1. 1.Visual Sciences CenterUniversity of ChicagoChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations