Skip to main content

Built for Speed: Pleistocene Climate Variation and the Origin of Human Culture

  • Chapter
Book cover Perspectives in Ethology

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Ethology ((PEIE,volume 13))

Abstract

The evolution of humans is a major event in the Earth’s biotic history. Never before has a single species of organism so dominated the planet. Hunting and gathering people penetrated to nearly every habitable scrap of the Earth’s surface by the end of the Pleistocene. Beginning about 10,000 years ago, food plant production, and its evolutionary sequelae, have made us a geochemical and geophysical force to be reckoned with. Most accounts of human origins take this ecological dominance as a sign of a qualitatively new and superior form of adaptation and ask, what evolutionary breakthrough led to the unique human adaptive complex—tool making, language, complex social organization, and other aspects of culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aiello, L. C, & Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36,199–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 5, 325–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allman, J. M. (1999). Evolving brains. New York: Scientific American Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anonymous (1871). Review of the descent of man and selection in relation to sex by Charles Darwin. The Quarterly Review, 131(216), 47–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, P. H. (1974). Darwin’s early and unpublished notebooks, In H. E. Gruber, Darwin on man (pp. 259–426), New York: Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barth, F. (1969). Ethnic groups and boundaries. Boston, MA: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basalla, G. (1988). The evolution of technology. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. (1987). Biological approaches to the study of behavioural development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10,1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behl, J. P., & Kennet, J. P. (1996). Brief interstadial events in the Santa Barbara Basin, NE Pacific, during the last 60kyr. Nature, 379, 243–246.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, E. W. (1964). A further loss of ground by Rattus rattus L. in the United Kingdom during 1956-61. Journal of Animal Ecology, 33, 371–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1991). Hunter-gatherers: Archaeological and evolutionary theory. New York:Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7,173–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare.Proceedings of the British Academy, 88,77–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16,125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). How microevolutionary processes give rise to history,In M. H. Nitecki and D. V. Nitecki (Eds.), History and evolution (pp. 179–210). Albany,NY: The State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1989). Social learning as an adaptation. Lectures on Mathematics in the Life Sciences, 20,1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology,2, 65–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. S. (1999). Paleoclimatology: Reconstructing climates of the quaternary (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandon, R. (1990). Adaptation and environment. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broecker, W. S. (1997). Thermohaline circulation, the achilles heel of our climate system: Will man-made C02 upset the current balance? Science, 278,1582–1588.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Broecker, W. S., & Denton, G. H. (1990). What drives glacial cycles? Scientific American,January, 49–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broecker, W. S., Peteet, D. M., & Rind, D. (1985). Does the ocean-atmosphere system have more than one stable mode of operation?Nature, 315,21–25.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cane, M. A. (1998). A role for the tropical Pacific. Science, 282, 59–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caro, T., & Hauser, M. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review of Biology, 67,151–174.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1973). Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation. Theoretical Population Biology, 4, 42–55.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chou, L.-S. (1989). Social transmission of food selection by rats. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Davis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies,M., Davies, H., & Davies, K. (1992). Humanking the gatherer-hunter: From earliest times to industry. Swanley, England: Myddle-Brockton.

    Google Scholar 

  • deMenocal, P. B. (1995). Plio-Pleistocene African climate. Science, 270, 53–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • deMenocal, P. B., & Bloemendal, J. (1995). Plio-Pleistocene climatic variability in subtropical Africa and the paleoenvironment of hominid evolution: A combined data-model approach. In E. S. Vrba, G. H. Denton, T. C. Partridge, and L. H. Burckle (Eds.), Paleoclimate and evolution with emphasis on human origins (pp. 262–298), New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (1978). The Tasmanians: The longest isolation, the simplest technology. Nature, 273,185–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ditlevsen, P. D., Svensmark, H., & Johnsen, S. (1996). Contrasting atmospheric and climate dynamics of the last-glacial and Holocene periods.Nature, 379, 810–812.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin, L. A. (1996). Copying and mate choice. In: C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 85–105). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1991). Functional significance of grooming in primates. Folia Primatologica, 57,121–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R., & commentators. (1995). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size, and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, J. F. (1981). The mammalian radiations: An analysis of trends in evolution, adaptation, and behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D. (1996). The temporal organization of language: Developmental and neuro-psychological aspects. In B. M. Velichkovsky and D. M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language (pp. 173–186). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of the mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, R. (1987). Another unique species: Patterns in human evolutionary ecology. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fragaszy, D. M., & Visalberghi, E. (1996). Social learning in monkeys: Primate “primacy” reconsidered. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 65–84). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, Jr. B. G. (1996). Social enhancement of food preferences in Norway rats: A brief review. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The Roots of Culture (pp. 49–64). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, Jr. B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals: History, definition, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory. In T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, (pp. 3–28), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef, Jr., B. G. (1977). Mechanisms for the social transmission of food preferences from adult to weanling rats. In: Barker, L. M., Best, M., and M. Domjan (Eds.), Learning mechanisms in food selection (pp. 123–150). Waco, TX: Baylor University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • GRIP (Greenland Ice-core Project Members). (1993). Climate instability during the last inter-glacial period recorded in the GRIP ice core. Nature, 364, 203–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grootes, P. M., Stuiver, M., White, J. W. C., Johnsen, S., & Jouzel, J. (1993). Comparison of oxygen isotope records from the GISP2 and GRIP Greenland ice cores. Nature, 366, 552–554.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical theory of social behavior, I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7,1–52.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harpending, H. C., Batzer, M. A., Gurvens, M., Jorde, L. B., Rogers, A. R., & Sherry, S. T. (1998). Genetic traces of ancient demography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95,1961–1967.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M, & Dawson G. R. (1990). A demonstration of observational learning using a bidirectional control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42B, 59–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., & Galef, B. G., Jr. (1996). Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, D. L. (1992). The particular-circumstance model of scientific explanation. In M. H. Nitecki and D. V. Nitecki (Eds.), History and evolution (pp. 69–80), Albany, NY: The State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317), Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, S., Gelatt, C. D., & Vecchi, M. P. (1983). Optimization by simulated annealing.Science, 220, 671–680.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. G. (1999). The human career: Human biological and cultural origins (2nd Ed.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krings, M., Stone, A., Schmitz, R. W., Krainitzki, H., Stoneking, M., & Pääbo, S. (1997). Neanderthal DNA sequences and the origin of modern humans. Cell, 60,19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krubitzer, L. (1995). The organization of the neocortex in mammals: are species differences really so different? Trends in the Neurosciences, 18, 408–417.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kummer, H., Daston, L., Gigerenzer, G., & Silk, J. B. (1997). The social intelligence hypothesis. In P. Weingart, S. D. Mitchell, P. J. Richerson, and S. Maasen (Eds.), Human by nature: Between biology and the social sciences (pp. 157–179). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (1993). Animal social learning: Toward a new theoretical approach. Perspectives in Ethology, 10,249–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Williams, K. (1998). Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology, 9, 493–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, H. H. (1977). Climatic history and the future. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L., & Palameta, B. (1988). Mechanisms, ecology, and population diffusion of socially learned, food finding behavior in feral pigeons. In T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 141–164). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre, L. (in press). Feeding innovations and their cultural transmission in bird populations. In C. Heyes and L. Huber (Eds.), Evolution of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, S. (1993). Climate change: Ice sheets, wayward winds and sea change. Nature, 365, 108–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manabe, S., & Stouffer, R. J. (1995). Simulation of abrupt climate change induced by freshwater input to the North Atlantic Ocean. Nature, 378,165–167.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marino, L. (1996). What can dolphins tell us about primate evolution? Evolutionary Anthropology, 5, 81–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. D. (1981). Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature, 293, 57–60.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, W. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Milankovitch, M. M. (1941). Canon of insolation and the ice-age problem. English Translation 1969, Israel Program for Scientific Translations, Washington, US Department of Commerce and National Science Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, B. R. (1996). The evolution of imitative learning. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 245–265). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm, F. (1975). Continental patterns of song variability in Zonotrichia capensis: Some possible ecological correlates. American Naturalist, 109, 605–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South.Boulder, CO: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Opdyke, N. (1995). Mammalian migration and climate over the past seven million years. In:E. S. Vrba, G. H. Denton, T. C. Partridge, and L. H. Burckle (Eds.), Paleoclimate and evolution with emphasis on human origins (pp. 109–114). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partridge, T. C., Bond, G. C, Hartnady, C. H. J., deMenocal, P. B., & Ruddiman. W. F. (1995).Climatic effects of late Neogene tectonism and vulcanism. In E. S. Vrba, G. H. Denton,T. C. Partridge, and L. H. Burckle, (Eds.), Paleoclimate and evolution with emphasis on human origins (pp. 8–23). New Haven, Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petit, J. R., Jouzel, J., Reynaud, D., Barkov, N. I., Barnola, X-M, Basile, I., Bender, M.,Chappellaz, J., Davis, M., Delaygue, G., Delmotte, M., Kotlyakov, V. M., Legrand,M., Lipenkov, V. Y., Lorius, C., Pépin, L., Ritz, C., Saltzman, E., & Stievenard, M.(1999). Climate and atmospheric history of the past 420,000 years from the Vostok ice core, Antarctica. Science, 399, 429–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potts, R. (1996). Humanity’s descent: The consequences of ecological instability. New York:William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raynaud, D., Jouzel, X, Barnola, X M., Jappellaz, J., Delmas, R. X, & Lorius, C. (1993). The ice record of greenhouse gases. Science, 259, 926–934.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (in press). Climate, culture and the evolution of cognition. In C.Heyes and L. Huber (Eds.), Evolution of cognition. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. R. (1989). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. R., & Harpending, H. C. (1992). Population growth makes waves in the distribution of pairwise genetic differences. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 9, 552–569.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Russon, A. E., & Galdikas, B. M. F. (1993). Imitation in free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans.Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107,147–161.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., & Winterhalder, B. (1992). Evolutionary ecology and human behavior. New York:Aldine De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 29–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steward, X H. (1955). Theory of culture change: The methodology of multilinear evolution.Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stringer, C., & Gamble, C. (1993). In search of the Neanderthals: Solving the puzzle of human origins. New York: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terkel, J. (1996). Cultural transmission of feeding behavior in the black rat (Rattus rattus). InC. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 17–47). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Thieme, H. (1997). Lower Paleolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature, 285, 807–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, N. S. (1995). Does language arise from a calculus of dominance? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, N., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1997). Introduction to evolutionary psychology. In P.Weingart, S. D. Mitchell, P. J. Richerson, and S. Maasen (Eds.), Human by nature:Between biology and the social sciences (pp. 212–238). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, P. (1981). The emergence of man in Africa and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 292, 43–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In C.M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, part I: Theoretical considerations. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10, 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, H. (1998). Cultural selection and genetic diversity in matrilineal whales. Science, 282, 708–1711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & Bryne, R. W. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & Custance, D. (1996). Studies of imitation in chimpanzees and children. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 291–318). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C, Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G, Wrangham, R. W., & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399,682–685.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall,T. (1996). An analysis of imitative learning in animals. In C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 221–243). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2000 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Richerson, P.J., Boyd, R. (2000). Built for Speed: Pleistocene Climate Variation and the Origin of Human Culture. In: Tonneau, F., Thompson, N.S. (eds) Perspectives in Ethology. Perspectives in Ethology, vol 13. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1221-9_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1221-9_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-5447-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-1221-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics