Advertisement

The Development and Function of Nepotism

Why Kinship Matters in Social Relationships
  • Warren G. Holmes
Part of the Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology book series (HBNE, volume 13)

Abstract

Each year since 1904, the Carnegie Hero Fund Commission has presented a medal and financial award to civilians who voluntarily risked their own lives to save another person’s. The Hero Fund came about because, according to its founder, Andrew Carnegie, “I do not expect to stimulate or create heroism by this fund, knowing well that heroic action is impulsive; but I do believe that, if the hero is injured in his bold attempt to serve or save his fellows, he and those dependent upon him should not suffer pecuniarily” ([Carnegie, 2000]). One type of hero who is ineligible for an award is the hero who saves a member of his or her own immediate family. Why would the Commission exclude from the list of deserving heroes those who save a close genetic relative? I suggest that the reason is quite straightforward: we humans take for granted that our kin will come to our aid in times of need; self-sacrifice for a relative like a child, a sibling, or a niece is part of our “nature” and thus does not merit pecuniary reward ([Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama, 1994]). Still, why should this be so? One of my aims in this chapter is to explain the ubiquitous and deeply ingrained nature of nepotism that permeates human and nonhuman social relationships. My analysis may help provide a deeper biological understanding of why saving a friend or stranger might merit financial reward, whereas saving a close relative is “natural” and therefore would not

Keywords

Social Preference Ground Squirrel Alarm Call Inclusive Fitness Social Play 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alberts, J. R. (1978). Huddling by rat pups: Group behavioral mechanisms of temperature regulation and energy conservation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 231–245PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alberts, J. R., & Cramer, C. P. (1988). Ecology and experience: Sources of means and meaning of developmental change. In E. M. Blass (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Volume 9, Developmental psychobiology and behavioral ecology (pp. 1–39). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  3. Alberts, J. R, & May, B. (1984). Nonnutritive, thermotactile induction of filial huddling in rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 161–181PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Alcock, J., & Sherman, P. (1994). The utility of the proximate-ultimate dichotomy in ethology. Ethology, 96, 58–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and human affairs Seattle, WA: University of Washington PressGoogle Scholar
  6. Alexander, R. D. (1990). Epigenetic rules and Darwinian algorithms. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 241–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Alexander, R. D., & Tinkle, D. W. (Eds.). (1981). Natural selection and social behavior New York: Chiron PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Antonovics, J., & van Tienderen, P. H. (1991). Ontoecogenophyloconstraints? The chaos of constraint terminology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 166–168PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Barash, D. P. (1989). Marmots: Social behavior and ecology Stanford, CA: Stanford University PressGoogle Scholar
  10. Bateson, P. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Animal Behaviour, 27, 470–486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bateson, P. (1981). Control of sensitivity to the environment during development. In K Immelmann, G. W. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral development (pp. 432–453). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  12. Bateson, P. (Ed.). (1983). Mate choice Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  13. Beecher, M. D. (1981). Development of parent-offspring recognition in birds. In R. K. Aslin, J R Alberts, & M. R. Petersen (Eds.), Development of perception (Vol. 1, pp. 45–65). Orlando, FL: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  14. Beecher, M. D. (1991). Successes and failures of parent-offspring recognition in animals. In P. G. Hepper (Ed.), Kin recognition (pp. 94–124). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  15. Beecher, M. D., Beecher, I. M., & Hahn, S. (1981). Parent-offspring recognition in bank swallows (Riparia riparia): II. Development and acoustic basis. Animal Behaviour, 29, 95–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bekoff, M. (1977). Mammalian dispersal and the ontogeny of individual behavioral phenotypes. American Naturalist, 111, 715–732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Bekoff, M. (1981). Mammalian sibling interactions: Genes, facilitative environments, and the coefficient of familiarity. In D.J. Gubernick & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.), Parental care in mammals (pp. 307–346). New York: Plenum PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Bekoff, M., & Byers, J. A. (1981). A critical reanalysis of the ontogeny and phylogeny of mammalian social and locomotor play: An ethological hornet’s nest. In K. Immelmann, G. W. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral development (pp. 296–337). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  19. Bekoff, M., & Byers, J. A. (1998). Animal play: Evolutionary,comparative, and ecological perspectives Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Bennett, B. (1987). Measures of relatedness. Ethology, 74, 219–236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Berman, C. M. (1982). The ontogeny of social relationship with group companions among free-ranging infant rhesus monkeys I. Social networks and differentiation. Animal Behaviour, 30, 149–162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Berman, C. M., Rasmussen, K L. R., & Suomi, S. J. (1997). Group size, infant development and social networks in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 53, 405–421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Blass, E. M. (1995). Ontogeny of ingestive behavior. In A. Morrison & S. Fluharty (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 16, pp. 1–51). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  24. Blass, E. M. (1999). The ontogeny of human face recognition: Orogustatory, visual and social influences. In: Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 35–65). Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  25. Blumberg, M. S., & Sokoloff, G. (1998). Thermoregulatory competence and behavioral expression in the young of altricial species-revisted. Developmental Psychobiology, 33, 107–123PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Blumstein, D. T., & Armitage, K. B. (1997). Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 53, 143–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Blumstein, D. T., & Armitage, K. B. (1998). Why do yellow-bellied marmots call? Animal Behaviour, 56, 1053–1055PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Blumstein, D. T., Steinmetz, J., Armitage, K B., & Daniel, J. C. (1997). Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: II. The importance of direct fitness. Animal Behaviour, 53, 173–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of avian imprinting: A review. Biological Reviews, 66, 303–345PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Breed, M. D., Welch, C. K, &Cruz, R. C. (1994). Kin discrimination within honey bee (Apis mellrfera) colonies: An analysis of the evidence. Behavioral Processes, 33, 25–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Brown, J. L. (1980). Fitness in complex avian social systems. In H. Mark (Ed.), Evolution of social behavior. Hypotheses and empirical tests (pp. 115–128). Weinheim, Germany: Verlag ChemieGoogle Scholar
  32. Brown, J. L. (1987). Helping and communal breeding in birds Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  33. Brunjes, P. C., & Alberts, J. R. (1979). Olfactory stimulation induces filial preferences for huddling in rat pups. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 548–555PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,67, 773–789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Carnegie. (2000). Carnegie Hero Fund Commission website, http://www.carnegiehero.org/history.shtml/history.shtml, revision 19 December 2000Google Scholar
  36. Chapais, B. (1992). The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among female primates. In A. Harcourt & F. de Waal (Eds.), Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals (pp. 29–59). New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  37. Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1999). Recognition of other individuals’ social relationships by female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 58,67–75PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Clutton-Brock, T (1991). The evolution of parental care Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  39. Cockburn, A. (1998). Evolution of helping behavior in cooperatively breeding birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics,29, 141–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Crozier, R. H., & Pamilo, P. (1996). Evolution of social insect colonies Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  41. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species London: MurrayGoogle Scholar
  42. Dawkins, M. S. (1995). Unravelling animal behaviour (2nd ed.). Essex, England: Longman Scientific & TechnicalGoogle Scholar
  43. Dawkins, R. (1979). Twelve misunderstandings of kin selection. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 51,184–200Google Scholar
  44. Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype Oxford: FreemanGoogle Scholar
  45. Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  46. Dewsbury, D. A. (1994). On the utility of the proximate-ultimate distinction in the study of animal behavior. Ethology, 96, 63–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Dunford, C. (1977). Social system of round-tailed ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 25, 885–906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Emlen, S. T. (1991). Evolution of cooperative breeding in birds and mammals. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology, 3rd ed. (pp. 301–337). London: Blackwell ScientificGoogle Scholar
  49. Emlen, S. T., & Wrege, P. H. (1992). Parent-offspring conflict and the recruitment of helpers among bee-eaters. Nature, 356, 331–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Fagen, R. (1981). Animal play behavior New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  51. Fairbanks, L. A. (1990). Reciprocal benefits of allomothering for female vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 40, 553–562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Fletcher, D. J. C., & Michener, C. D. (1987). Kin recognition in animals Chichester, England: WileyGoogle Scholar
  53. Fraser, D., & Thompson, B. K. (1991). Armed sibling rivalry among suckling piglets. Behavioral Ecology and.Sociobiology, 29, 9–15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Freeberg, F. M. (1996). Assortative mating in captive cowbirds is predicted by social experience. Animal Behaviour, 52, 1129–1142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Gamboa, G.J., Reeve, H. K., Ferguson, I. D., & Wacker, T. L. (1986). Nestmate recognition in social wasps: The origin and acquisition of recognition odours. Animal Behaviour, 34, 685–695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Gamboa, G. J., Foster, R. L., Scope, J. A., & Bitterman, A. M. (1991). Effects of stage of colony cycle, context, and intercolony distance on conspecific tolerance by paper wasps (Polistes fuscatus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29, 87–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Gamboa, G. J Reeve, H. K., & Holmes, W. G. (1991). Conceptual issues and methodology in kin-recognition research: A critical discussion. Ethology, 88, 109–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Gordon, D. M. (1992). Phenotypic plasticity. In E. Fox Keller & E. A. Lloyd (Eds.), Keywords in evolutionary biology (pp. 255–262). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
  59. Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  60. Govindarajulu, P., Hunte, W., Vermeer, L. A., & Horrocks, J. A. (1993). The ontogeny of social play in a feral troop of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus arthiope sabaeus): The function of early play. International Journal of Primatology, 14, 701–719CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Grafen, A. (1984). Natural selection, kin selection and group selection. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology, 2nd ed. (pp. 62–84). Sunderland, MA: SinauerGoogle Scholar
  62. Grinnell, J., Packer, C., & Pusey, A. E. (1995), Cooperation in male lions: Kinship, reciprocity or mutual-ism? Animal Behaviour, 49, 95–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Gubernick, D. J., & Klopfer, P. H (Eds.). (1981). Parental care in mammals New York: Plenum PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Hallman, J. P., McGowan, K J., & Woolfenden, G. E. (1994). Role of helpers in the sentinel behavior of the florida scrub jay (Aphelocoma c. coerulescens). Ethology, 97, 119–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7,1–52PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Hanken, J., & Sherman, P. W. (1981). Multiple paternity in Belding’s ground squirrel litters. Science, 212, 351–353PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Hanber, M. E, & Sherman, P. W. (1998). Nepotism and marmot alarm calling. AnimalBehaviour, 56,1049–1052Google Scholar
  68. Heth, G., Todrank, J., & Johnston, R. E. (1998). Kin recognition in golden hamsters: Evidence for phenotype matching. Animal Behaviour, 56, 409–417PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Hinde, R. A. (1979). What do we mean by a relationship? In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Towards understanding relationships (pp. 14–39). London: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  70. Hinde, R. A. (Ed.). (1983). Primate social relationships Oxford: Blackwell ScientificGoogle Scholar
  71. Holekamp, K. E. (1983). Proximal mechanism of natal dispersal in Belding’s ground squirrel (Spermophilus beldingi beldingi) Ph.D. thesis, University of California, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  72. Holekamp, K. E. (1986). Proximal causes of natal dispersal in Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Ecological Monographs, 56, 365–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Holmes, W. G. (1984). Sibling recognition in thirteen-lined ground squirrels: Effects of genetic related-ness, rearing association, and olfaction. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 14, 225–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Holmes, W. G. (1986). Kin recognition by phenotype matching in female Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 34, 38–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Holmes, W. G (1988). Kinship and the development of social preferences. In E. M, Blass (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Volume 9, Developmental psychobiology and behavioral ecology (pp. 389–413). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  76. Holmes, W. G. (1990). Parent-offspring recognition in mammals: A proximate and ultimate perspective. In N. A. Krasnegor & R. S. Bridges (Eels.), Mammalian parenting (pp. 441–460). New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  77. Holmes, W. G. (1994). The development of littermate preferences in juvenile Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1071–1084CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Holmes, W. G. (1995). The ontogeny of littermate preferences in juvenile golden-mantled ground squirrels: Effects of rearing and relatedness. Animal Behaviour, 50, 309–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Holmes, W. G. (1997). Temporal aspects in the development of Belding’s ground squirrels’ litter-mate preferences. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1323–1336PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Holmes, W. G., & Mateo, J. M. (1998). How mothers influence the development of littermate preferences in Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1555–1570PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Holmes, W. G., & Sherman, P. W. (1982). The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels. American Zoologist, 22, 491–517Google Scholar
  82. Holmes, W. G, & Sherman, P. W. (1983). Kin recognition in animals American Scientist, 71, 46–55Google Scholar
  83. Hoogland, J. L. (1983). Nepotism and alarm calling in the black-tailed prairie dog (Cynomys ludovicianus). Animal Behaviour, 31, 472–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Hoogland, J. L. (1985). Infanticide in prairie dogs Lactating females kill offspring of close kin. Science, 230, 1037–1040PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Hoogland, J. L. (1995). The black-tailed prairie dog Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
  86. Immelmann, K, & Suomi, S.J. (1981). Sensitive phases in development. In K. Immelmann, G. W. Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral development: The Bielefeld Interdisciplinary Project (pp. 395–430). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  87. Jenkins, S. H., & Eshelman, B. D. (1984). Spermophilus beldingi. Mammalian Species, 221, 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Jenks, S. M, Weldele, M. L., Frank, L. G., & Glickman, S. E. (1995). Acquisition of matrilineal rank in captive spotted hyaenas: Emergence of a natural social system in peer-reared animals and their offspring. Animal Behaviour, 50, 893–904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Johnson, R. C. (1996). Attributes of Carnegie medalists performing acts of heroism and of the recipients of these acts. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 355–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Keller, L. (1997). Indiscriminate altruism: Unduly nice parents and siblings. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 12, 99–103PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Kempenaers, B., & Sheldon, B. C. (1996). Why do male birds not discriminate between their own and extra-pair offspring? Animal Behaviour, 51 1165–1173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Koenig, W. D., & Mumme, R. L. (1987). Population ecology of the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  93. Komdeur, J., & Hatchwell, B. J. (1999). Kin recognition: Function and mechanism in avian societies. Trends in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology,14, 237–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Krasnegor, N. A., & Bridges, R. S. (Eds.). (1990). Mammalian parenting New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  95. Kruijt, J. P., & Meeuwissen, G B. (1993). Consolidation and modification of sexual preferences in adult male zebra finches. Netherlands Journal of Zoology, 43, 68–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Kummer, H. (1978). On the value of social relationships to non-human primates: A heuristic scheme. Social Science Information, 17, 687–705CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Lacey, E. A., & Sherman, P. W. (1997). Cooperative breeding in naked mole-rats: Implications for vertebrate and invertebrate sociality. In N. G. Solomon & J. A, French (Eds.), Cooperative breeding in mammals (pp. 267–301). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  98. Lacy, R. C., & Sherman, P. W. (1983). Kin recognition by phenotype matching. American Naturalist, 121, 489–512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Lavinia, G., & Terranova, M. L. (1998). The developmental psychobiology of behavioural plasticity in mice: The role of social experiences in the family unit. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 197–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Lecanuet, J.-P., Fifer, W. P., Krasnegor, W. P. F., & Smotherman, W. P. (Eds.). (1995). Fetal development: A psychobiological perspective Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  101. Lee, P. C. (1983). Play as a means for developing relationships. In R. A. Hinde (Ed.), Primate social relationships (pp. 82–89). Oxford: Blackwell ScientificGoogle Scholar
  102. Lee, P. C. (1987). Allomotheríng among African elephants. Animal Behaviour, 35, 278–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In L. R. Aron-son, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior (pp. 17–52). San Francisco: FreemanGoogle Scholar
  104. Leti, G. (1669). II nipotismo di Roma, or, The history of the popes nephews: From the time of Sixtus IV anno 1471, to the death of the late Pope Alexander VII, anno 1667 London: John StarkeyGoogle Scholar
  105. Lickliter, R., & Gottlieb, G. (1985). Social interactions with siblings is necessary for the visual imprinting of species-specific maternal preference in ducklings. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 371–378PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Lickliter, R., & Gottlieb, G. (1986). Training ducklings in broods interferes with maternal imprinting. Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 555–566PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Loeschcke, V. (Ed.). (1987). Genetic constraints on adaptive evolution: Berlin: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Manson, J. H. (1999). Infant handling in wild Cebus capucinus: Testing bonds between females? Animal Behaviour, 57, 911–921PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Martin, P., & Caro, T. M. (1985). On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. In J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, M.-C. Busnel, & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vo1.15, pp. 59–103), New York: Academic Press CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Mateo, J. M., & Holmes, W. G. (1997). Development of alarm-call responses in Belding’s ground squirrels: The role of dams. Animal Behaviour, 59, 509–524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Maxwell, C. S., & Morton, M. L. (1975). Comparative thermoregulatory capabilities of neonatal ground squirrels. Journal of Mammalogy, 56, 821–828PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature, 201, 1145–1147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Maynard Smith, J., Burlan, R., Kauffman, S., Alberch, P., Campbell, J., Goodwin, B., Lande, R., Raup, D., & Wolpert, L. (1985). Developmental constraints and evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 60, 265–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Meaney, M. J., Stewart, J., & Beatty, W. W. (1985). Sex differences in social play: The socialization of sex roles. In J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, M.-C. Busnel, & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 15, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Michener, G. R. (1981). Ontogeny of spatial relationships and social behaviour in juvenile Richardson’s ground squirrels. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 59, 1666–1676CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Michener, G. R (1983). Kin identification, matriarchies, and the evolution of sociality in ground-dwelling sciurids. In J. F. Eisenberg &D. G. Kleiman (Eds.), Advances in the study of mammalian behavior (pp. 528–572). Shippensburg, PA: American Society of MammalogistsGoogle Scholar
  117. Mock, D. W., & Parker, G. A. (1997). The evolution of sibling rivalry Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  118. Mock, D. W., & Parker, G. A. (1998). Siblicide, family conflict and the evolutionary limits of selfishness. Animal Behaviour, 56, 1–10PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Morton, M. L., & Gallup, J. S. (1975). Reproductive cycle of the Belding ground squirrel (Spermophilus beldingi beldingz): Seasonal and age differences. Great Basin Naturalist, 35, 427–433Google Scholar
  120. Mumme, R. L., & Koenig, W. D. (1991). Explanations for avian helping behavior. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 6, 343–344PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Murie, J. O., & Michener, G. R. (1984). The biology of ground-dwelling squirrels Lincoln: University of Nebraska PressGoogle Scholar
  122. Nij bout, H. F., & Emlen, D.J. (1998). Competition among body parts in the development and evolution of insect morphology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, 3685–3689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Nunes, S., Muecke, E. M., Anthony, J. A., & Batterbee, A. S. (1999). Endocine and energetic mediation of play behavior in free-living Belding’s ground squirrels. Hormones and Behavior, 36, 153–165PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. O’Gara, B. (1969). Unique aspects of reproduction in the female pronghorn (Antilocapra americana). American Journal of Anatomy, 125, 217–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Packer, C., Gilbert, D. A., Pusey, A. E., &O’Brien, S. J. (1991), A molecular genetic analysis of kinship and cooperation in African lions. Nature, 351, 562–565CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Parker, P. G., Waite, T. A., &Decker, M. D. (1995). Kinship and association in communally roosting black vultures. Animal Behaviour, 49, 395–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Panksepp, J., Siviy, S., & Normansell, L. A. (1984). The psychobiology of play: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 8, 465–492PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Pellis, S. M., Field, E. F., Smith, L. K., & Pellis, V. C. (1997). Multiple differences in the play fighting of male and female rats. Implications for the causes and functions of play. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 21, 105–120PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Pellis, S. M., & Iwaniuk, A. N. (1999). The roles of phylogeny and sociality in the evolution of social play in muroid rodents. Animal Behaviour, 58, 361–373PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Pereira, M. E. (1995). Development and social dominance among group-living primates. American Journal of Primatology, 37, 143–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Pereira, M. E., & Altmann, J. (1985). Development of social behavior in free-living nonhuman primates. In E. S. Watt (Ed.), Nonhuman primate models for human growth and development (pp. 217–309). New York: LissGoogle Scholar
  132. Pereira, M. E, & Fairbanks, L. A. (Eds.). (1993). Juvenile primates New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  133. Pfenning, D. W., Sherman, P. W., &Collins, J. P. (1994). Kin recognition and cannibalism in polyphenic salamanders. Behavioral Ecology, 5, 225–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Pfister, J. F., Cramer, C. P., Sc Blass, E. M. (1986). Suckling in rats extended by continuous living with dams and their preweanling litters. Animal Behaviour, 34, 415–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Poirier, F. E., & Smith, E. O. (1974). Socializing functions of primate play. American Zoologist, 14, 275–287Google Scholar
  136. Porter, R. H., Moore, J. D., & White, D. M. (1981). Food sharing by sibling vs nonsibling spiny mice (Acomys cahirinus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8, 207–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Porter, R. H., Tepper, V. J., & White, D. M. (1981). Experiential influences on the development of huddling preferences and “sibling” recognition in spiny mice. Developmental Psychobiology, 14, 375–382PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Porter, R. H., Cernoch, J. M., & McLaughlin, F. J. (1983). Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiology and Behavior, 30, 151–154PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Pusey, A. E., & Packer, C. (1987). The evolution of sex-biased dispersal in lions. Behaviour, 101, 275–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Robinson, S. R., & Smotherman, W. P. (1991). Fetal learning: Implications for the development of kin recognition. In P. G. Hepper (Ed.), Kin recognition (pp. 308–334). Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  141. Rose, M. R., & Lauder, G. V. (Eds.). (1996). Adaptation San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  142. Rubenstein, D. I., & Wrangham, R W. (Eds.). (1986). Ecological aspects of social evolution Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  143. Scheiner, S. M. (1993). Genetics and evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 24, 35–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Schwagmeyer, P. L. (1980). Alarm calling behavior of the thirteen-lined ground squirrel, Spermophilus tridecemlineatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 7, 195–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Schwagmeyer, P. L. (1990). Ground squirrel reproductive behavior and mating competition: A comparative perspective. In D. A. Dewsbury (Ed.), Contemporary issues in comparative psychology (pp. 175–196). Sunderland, MA: SinauerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Sherman, P. W. (1977). Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science, 197, 1246–1253PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Sherman, P. W. 1980a. The meaning of nepotism American Naturalist, 116, 604–606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Sherman, P. W. 1980b. The limits of ground squirrel nepotism. In G. W. Barlow & J. Silverberg (Eds.), Sociobiology: Beyond nature/nurture? (pp. 505–544). Boulder, CO: Westview PressGoogle Scholar
  149. Sherman, P. W. 1981a. Reproductive competition and infanticide in Belding’s ground squirrels and other animals. In R. D. Alexander & D. W. Tinkle (Eds.), Natural selection and social behavior (pp. 311–331). New York: Chiron PressGoogle Scholar
  150. Sherman, P. W. 1981b. Kinship, demography, and Belding’s ground squirrel nepotism. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8, 251–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Sherman, P. W. (1985). Alarm calls of Belding’s ground squirrels to aerial predators: Nepotism or self-preservation? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 17, 313–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Sherman, P. W., & Morton, M. L. (1984). Demography of Belding’s ground squirrels. Ecology, 65, 1617–1628CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Sherman, P. W., Jarvis, J. U. M., &Alexander, R D. (Eds.). (1991). The biology of the naked mole-rat Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  154. Sherman, P. W., Reeve, H. K., & Pfennig, D. W. (1997). Recognition systems. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioral ecology, 4th ed., (pp. 69–96). Oxford: Blackwell ScientificGoogle Scholar
  155. Shields, W. M. (1980). Ground squirrel alarm calls: Nepotism or parental care? American Naturalist, 116, 599–603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Silk, J. B. (1999). Why are infants so attractive to others? The form and function of infant handling in bonnet macaques. Animal Behaviour, 57, 1021–1032PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Sluckin, W. (1964). Imprinting and early learning Chicago: AldineGoogle Scholar
  158. Smale, L., Nunes, S., & Holekamp, K. E. (1997). Sexually dimorphic dispersal in mammals: Patterns, causes, and consequences. In P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. T. Snowdon, & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 26, pp. 181–250). New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  159. Small, M. F., & Smith, D. G. (1981). Interactions with infants by full siblings, paternal half-siblings, and nonrelatives in a group of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). AmericanJournal ofPrimatology, I, 91–94Google Scholar
  160. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., & Struhsaker, T, T. (Eds.). (1987). Primate societies Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
  161. Solomon, N. G., & French, J. A. (Eds.). (1997). Cooperative breeding in mammals Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  162. Steiner, A. L. (1971). Play activity of Columbian ground squirrels. Zeitschriftfür Tierpsychologie, 28, 247–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Sun, L., & Muller-Schwarze, D. (1997). Sibling recognition in the beaver: A field test for phenotype matching. Animal Behaviour, 54, 493–502PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  164. Thompson, K. V. (1996). Play-partner preferences and the function of social play in infant sable antelope, Hippotragus niger. Animal Behaviour, 52, 1143–1155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  165. Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264Google Scholar
  167. Vehrencamp, S. L. (1979). The role of individual, kin, and group selection in the evolution of sociality. In P. Marler & J. G. Vandenbergh (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Volume 3, Social behavior and communication (pp. 351–394). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  168. de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Macaque social culture: Development and perpetuation of affiliative networks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 147–154PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Waldman, B., Frumhoff, P. C., & Sherman, P. W. (1988). Problems of kin recognition. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 3, 8–13PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Waterman, J. M. (1988). Social play in free-ranging Columbian ground squirrels, Spermophilus columbianus. Ethology, 77, 225–236Google Scholar
  171. West, M.J., King, A. P., & Arberg, A. A. (1988). The inheritances of niches: The role of ecological legacies in ontogeny. In E. M. Blass (Ed.), Handbook of behavioral neurobiology, Volume 9, Developmental psychobiology and behavioral ecology (pp. 41–62). New York: Plenum PressGoogle Scholar
  172. West-Eberhard, M. J. (1975). The evolution of social behavior by kin selection. Quarterly Review of Biology, 50, 1–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  173. West-Eberhard, M. J. (1989). Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 20, 249–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. Westneat, D. R, & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Parentage and the evolution of parental behavior. Behavioral Ecology, 4, 66–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection Princeton, NJ: Princeton University PressGoogle Scholar
  176. Wu, H. M. H., Holmes, W. G., Medina, S. R., & Sackett, G. P. (1980). Kin preference in infant Macaca nemestrina. Nature, 285, 225–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  177. Zahavi, A. (1977). The testing of a bond. Animal Behaviour, 25, 246–247CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Warren G. Holmes
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of MichiganAnn Arbor

Personalised recommendations