Foreign Faces: A Voyage to the Land of EEPICA

  • Robert J. Barrett
  • Mary Katsikitis

Abstract

In 1967 the psychologist Paul Ekman, of the Langley Porter Neuropsychiatrie Institute in San Francisco, visited the South Eastern Highlands of New Guinea to study the facial expression of emotion and its recognition among the Fore. He was interested in the Fore because they were a preliterate, geographically remote people. They had had very little contact with the Western world, not two decades before having been an isolated, Neolithic, material culture (Ekman & Friesen, 1971, p. 125) although Gajdusek’s investigations into the degenerative disorder, Kuru, in this group had brought them to international focus (Gajdusek, 1963; Sorenson & Gajdusek, 1966).

Keywords

Burning Depression Europe Schizophrenia Argentina 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Arkley, L. (2000). The hated protector. The story of Charles Wightman Sievwright, protector of aborigines 1839–42. QLD: Orbit Press.Google Scholar
  2. Arnold, M. (1960). Emotion and personality. Vol. 1. Psychological aspects. Vol. 2. Neurological and physiological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Barrett, R. J. & Lucas, R. H. (1993). The skulls are cold, the house is hot: Interpreting depths of meaning in Iban therapy. Man, 28, 573–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bateson, G., and Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character, a photographic analysis. New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  5. Bell, C. (1844). The anatomy and philosophy of expression as connected with the fine arts. London, John Murray.Google Scholar
  6. Birdwhistell, R.L. (1963). The kinesic level in the investigation of emotions. In P.H. Knapp (Ed.), Expression of the Emotions in Man (pp. 123–139). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  7. Burrows, E.G. (1963). Flower in my ear: Arts and ethos of Ifaluk atoll.Seattle, University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, I. (1995) Scars in the landscape: A register of massacre sites in western Victoria, 1803–1859. Australian Institute of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies Report Series. Canberra: Aboriginal Studies Press.Google Scholar
  9. Crawford, J., Kippax, S., Onyx, J., Gault, U., and Benton, P.(1992). Emotion and gender: Constructing meaning from memory. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  10. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Darwin, C. (1999). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Harper Collins: UK.Google Scholar
  12. Duchenne, B. (1862). Mècanisme de la physionomie humaine; ou, analyse électrophysiologique de expression des passions. Paris: Balliere.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ekman, P. (Ed). (1973). Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
  14. Ekman, P., and Friesen, W.V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ekman, P., and Friesen, W.V. (1975). Unmasking the face. New Jersey, Prentice-Hall Inc.Google Scholar
  16. Ekman, P., Sorenson, E.R., and Friesen, W.V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164, 86–88.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Frank, M. (2002). Getting to know your patient: How facial expression reveals emotion. In M.Katsikitis (Ed.). The Human Face: Measurement and Meaning (pp. 255–283). Dordrecht: Springer Science+Business Media New York.Google Scholar
  18. Gajdusek, D.C. (1963). Kuru epidemiological patrol from the New Guinea highlands to Papua: August 21, 1957 to November 10, 1957. BethesdaMd: Study of Child Growth and Development and Disease Patterns in Primitive Cultures. National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Stroke, National nstitute of Health.Google Scholar
  19. Gellhorn, E. (1964). Motion and emotion: The role of proprioception in the physiology and pathology of the emotions. Psychological Review, 71, 457–472.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, New York: Doubleday, Anchor Books.Google Scholar
  21. Good, B.J. (1994). Medicine, rationality, and experience: An anthropological perspective. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Goodenough, F.L. (1932). Expression of the emotions in a blind-deaf child. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 328–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences. Beverley Hills, Sage.Google Scholar
  24. Hopper, K. (in press). Interrogating “culture” in the WHO international studies of schizophrenia. In Jenkins, J., & Barrett, R. (Eds.), The Edge of Experience: Culture, Subjectivity, and Schizophrenia. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Izard, C.E. (1971). The face of emotion.New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
  26. Jansz, J. (2000). Masculine identity and restrictive emotionality. In A.H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological Perspectives (pp. 166–186). Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jensen, G. D., & Suryarni, L. K. (1992).The Balinese people. A reinvestigation of character.New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Kappas, A. (2002). What facial activity can and cannot tell us about emotions. In M. Katsikitis (Ed.),The human face: Measurement and meaning (pp. 215–234). Dordrecht: Springer Science+Business Media New York.Google Scholar
  29. LaBarre W. (1947). The cultural bias of emotions and gestures.Journal of Personality, 16, 49–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lakoff G., & Johnson, M. (1980).Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  31. Landis, C. (1924). Studies of emotion reactions: l.A preliminary study of facial expression.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 447–498.Google Scholar
  32. Leach, E. (1972). The influence of cultural context on nonverbal communication in man. In R.Hinde (Ed.),Nonverbal Communication (pp 315–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Lucas, R. H. & Barrett, R. J. (1995). Interpreting culture and psychopathology: Primitivist themes in cross-cultural debate.Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 19, 287–326.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lutz, C. (1988).Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll & their challenge to western theory.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  35. Lutz, C., & White, G.M. (1986). The anthropology of emotions.Annual Review of Anthropology, 15, 405–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Matsumoto, D. (1991). Cultural influences on facial expressions of emotion,Southern Communication Journal, 56, 128–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Matsumoto, D., and Ekman, P. (1989). American-Japanese cultural differences in intensity ratings of facial expressions of emotion.Motivation and Emotion, 13, 143–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mead, M. (1975). Review of Darwin and facial expression.Journal of Communication, 25, 209–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Obeyesekere, G. (1985). Buddhism, depression, and the work of culture in Sri Lanka. In A.Kleinman, & B.Good (Eds.),Culture and Depression (pp. 134–152). Berkeley: University of California.Google Scholar
  40. Pringle, R. (1970).Rajahs and rebels: The Iban of Sarawak under Brooke rule, 1841–1941. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  41. Reynolds, H. (1987).Frontier: Reports from the edge of white settlement. Sydney: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
  42. Rosaldo, M.Z. (1980)Knowledge and passion: Illongot notions of self and social life. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rose, A. M. (1962) A systematic summary of symbolic interaction theory. In A.M.Rose (Ed.),Human Behavior and Social Processes: An Interactionist Approach (pp. 3–19) London: Houghton Mifflin Co.Google Scholar
  44. Runciman, S. (1960).The white rajahs: A history of Sarawak from 1841–1946. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Russell, J.A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schieffelin, E. (1976).The sorrow of the lonely and the burning of the dancers. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  47. Schieffelin, E. (1985). Anger, grief, and shame: Toward aKaluli ethnopsychology. In G.M. White & J. Kirkpatrick (Eds.),Person, Self and Experience: Exploring Pacific Ethnopsychologies (pp. 168–182). Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  48. Schlosberg, H. (1941). A scale for the judgement of facial expressions.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sorenson, E.R., and Gajdusek, D.C. (1966). The study of child behavior and development in primitive cultures. A research archive for ethnopediatric film investigations of styles in the patterning of the nervous system.Pediatrics 37 (1, Pt. 2).Google Scholar
  50. Swift, J. (1984).Gulliver’s travels. London, Herbert.Google Scholar
  51. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations.Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 45–81.Google Scholar
  52. Taussig, M. (1993).Mimesis and alterity: A particular history of the senses. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
  53. Tomkins, S.S. (1962).Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 1. The positive affects. New York, Springer.Google Scholar
  54. Triandis, H.C., Botempo, R., Villareal, M.J., Asai, M., and Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relationships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 323–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Woodworth, R.S. (1938).Experimental psychology. New York, Holt.Google Scholar
  56. Young, A. (1993). Editorial: W. H. R. Rivers and the anthropology of psychiatry. Social Science and Medicine, 36, iii-vii.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert J. Barrett
    • 1
  • Mary Katsikitis
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of AdelaideAustralia

Personalised recommendations