Advertisement

Reading Disabilities in a Language with Transparent Orthography

  • J. E. Jiménez González
Part of the Neuropsychology and Cognition book series (NPCO, volume 20)

Abstract

The aim of the present chapter is to provide some empirical evidence about underlying mechanisms that are involved in the explanation of reading disabilities in a language with transparent orthography (i.e., Spanish). Previously, we analyze some phonological and orthographical specificities of English and Spanish in order to describe their implications on psycholinguistic processing. Specifically, our analysis is centered on word recognition processes, therefore we describe some psycholinguistic parameters which have been used in Spanish studies in order to determine to what extent some reading models (e.g., dual models) are functional in a transparent orthography. If reading mechanisms are the same for different alphabetical writing systems, then the pattern of results found in English should be expected in Spanish as well. Likewise, taking into account that dual-route theory has been used to explain the mechanisms underlying developmental dyslexics’ word reading, we review some research carried out in Spanish which has been designed to test whether the phenotypic pattern in a transparent orthography matches that found in English. But before we discuss these results we also review some problems related to the definition of reading disorders. The definition for developmental reading disorder relies on a discrepancy between expected and actual achievement. One of the most important tests for the credibility of and justification for the discrepancy definition of dyslexia is its ability to identify a unique group of individuals with reading problems that is different from ordinary poor readers who have no discrepancy. We review some bibliography literature on this topic in English, and we provide additional empirical evidence from Spanish studies which demonstrate that the IQ discrepancy based definition for dyslexia should be abandoned in favor of a definition which incorporates the phonological deficit hypothesis. On the other hand, we also review some studies about the relative influence that different forms of orthographic units (e.g., syllables, onset-rime, etc.) on the explanation of reading disabilities as a function of orthographic systems. And finally, we provide empirical data relevant to the issue whether specific reading disabilities fit a developmental lag or deficit model in a transparent orthography.

Keywords

Word Recognition Phonological Awareness Lexical Decision Task Poor Reader Reading Disability 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Backman, J., Bruck, M., Herbert, M., & Seidenberg, M. (1984). Acquisition and use of spelling-sound correspondences in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 114–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (1991). Visual word recognition: Evidence for strategic control of lexical and nonlexical routines in oral reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 644–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boder, E., & Jarrico, S. (1982). The Boder test of reading and spelling patterns: A diagnostic screening test for subtypes of reading disabilities. Orlando, FL: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
  5. Bowey, J., Cain, M., & Ryan, S. (1992). A reading-level design study of phonological skills underlying fourth-grade children’s word reading difficulties. Child Development, 63, 999–1011.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 285–298.Google Scholar
  7. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. I.A.R.L.D. Monographs 1. Ann Arbor. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bryant, P. E., & Goswami, U. (1986). Strengths and weaknesses of the reading level design: A comment on Backman, Mamen, and Ferguson. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 101–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L., & Crossland, M. (1990). Rhyme, alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Development Psychology, 26, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bryson, S. E., & Werker, J. F. (1989). Toward understanding the problems in severely disabled readers: Part I. Vowel errors. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Carreiras, M., Alvarez, C., & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish, Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Carrillo, M. S. (1994). Development of phonological awareness and reading acquisition. A study in Spanish language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 279–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of Information Processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  16. Coltheart, M. (1980). Reading, phonological recoding and deep dyslexia. In M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson, & J.C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep Dyslexia (pp. 197–226). London-New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  17. Cuetos, F. (1989). Lectura y escritura de palabras a través de la ruta fonológica [Reading and spelling using a phonological route]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 45, 71–84.Google Scholar
  18. Defior, S., Justicia, F., & Martos, F. (1996). The influence of lexical and sublexical variables in normal and poor Spanish readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8,487–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. de Vega, M., & Carreiras, M. (1989). The role of graphemic frequency in visual word processing. Communication presented at the 3rd European Conference for Learning and Instruction. Madrid.Google Scholar
  20. de Vega, M., Carreiras, M., Gutiérrez, M., & Alonso-Quecuty, M. L. (1990). Lectura y comprensión. Una perspectiva cognitiva[Reading and comprehension. A cognitive approach]. Madrid: Alianza Editorial.Google Scholar
  21. Domínguez, A. & Cuetos, F. (1992). Desarrollo de las habilidades de reconocimiento de palabras en nifios con distintas competencias lectoras [Word recognition and reading disabilities]. Cognitiva, 4, 193–208.Google Scholar
  22. Domínguez, A., Cuetos, F., & de Vega, M. (1993). Efectos diferenciales de la frecuencia silabica positional: dependencia del tipo de prueba y características de los estímulos [Effects of positional frequency of syllables as a function of task and stimuli]. Estudios de Psicología, 50,5–31.Google Scholar
  23. Ehri, L. C. (1975). Word consciousness in readers and prereaders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 204–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ehri, L. C., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7,295–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L.S. (1983). Development of word identification speed in skilled and less-skilled beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 3–18.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multidimensional comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Garcia-Albea, J. E., Sánchez, C. R., & del Viso-Pabon, S. (1982). Efectos de la frecuencia de uso en el reconocimiento de palabras [Effects of word frequency in word recognition]. Investigacion Psicologica, 1, 24–63.Google Scholar
  28. Goodman, K. S. (1976a). What we know about reading. In O. D. Allen, & D. J. Watson (Eds.), Findings of research in miscue analysis: Classroom implications. Urbana Illinois US. National Council of Teachers of English (NCTE).Google Scholar
  29. Goodman, K. S. (1976b). Behind the eyes: What happens in reading. In H. Singere, & R. B. Rudell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 497–508). Newark, N.J.: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
  30. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Humphreys, G. W., & Evett, L. J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Jiménez, J. E. (1997). A reading-level match study of phonemic processes underlying reading disabilities in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 23–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jiménez, J. E., & Hernández-Valle, I. (1999). A Spanish perspective on LD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 267–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jiménez, J. E., & Hernández-Valle, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 44–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jiménez, J. E., & Ortiz, M. R. (2000). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition in the Spanish language. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 3, 37–46.Google Scholar
  36. Jiménez, J. E., & Rodrigo, M. (1994). Is it true that the differences in reading performance between students with and without LD cannot be explained by IQ ? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 155–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jiménez, J. E.; Guzmán, R., & Artiles, C. (1997). Efectos de la frecuencia silabica positional en el reconocimiento visual de palabras y aprendizaje de la lectura [Effects of positional syllable frequency in visual word recognition and learning to read]. Cognitiva, 1, 3–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jiménez, J. E., Alvarez, C. J., Estévez, A., & Hernández-Valle, I. (2000). Onset-rime units in visual word recognition in Spanish normal readers and children with reading disabilities. Learning Disability Research & Practice, 15, 135–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: from eye fixation to comprehension. Psychology Review, 87, 329–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lenz, B. K., & Hughes, CH. A. (1990). A word identification strategy for adolescents with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 149–163.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Morais, J. (1995). Do orthographic and phonological peculiarities of alphabetically written languages influence the course of literacy acquisition ? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7,1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. National Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities (1994). Collective perspectives on issues affecting learning disabilities. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
  43. Nicholson, T. (1991). Do children read words better in context or in lists? A classic study revisited. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 444–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Olson, R. K. (1994). Language deficits in “specific” reading disability. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 895–916). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  45. Patterson, K., & Coltheart, V. (1987). Phonological processes in reading: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 422–447). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  46. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Perfetti, C. A. (1986). Continuities in reading acquisition, reading skill and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Perfetti, C. A., Bell, L. C., & Delaney, S. M. (1988). Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 59–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1987). Eye movements in reading: A tutorial review. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention & performance XIII: The psychology of reading (pp. 327–362). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  50. Rodrigo, M, & Jiménez, J. E. (1999). An analysis of the word naming errors of normal readers and reading disabled children in Spanish. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 180–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Rodrigo, M., & Jimenez, J. E. (2000). IQ vs phonological recoding skill in explaining differences between poor readers and normal readers in word recognition: Evidence from a naming task. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 129–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1991). Reading by Analogy in a Shallow Orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 471–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems. Cognition, 19, 1–30.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Siegel, L. S. (1986). Phonological deficits in children with a reading disability. Canadian Journal of Special Education, 2, 45–54Google Scholar
  55. Siegel, L. S. (1989). I.Q. is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 469–478.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Siegel, L. S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 618–629.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Snowling, M. J. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 294–305.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and garden variety poor reader: The phonological-core variance-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21,590–604.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Toth, G., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). A critical evaluation of the IQ-achievement discrepancy based definition of dyslexia. In K. P. van den Bos., L. S. Siegel., D. J. Bakker., & D. L. Share. (Eds.), Currents directions in dyslexia research (pp. 45–70). Linsse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
  60. Treiman, R. (1992). The role of intrasyllabic units in learning to read and spell. In P. B. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp.65–106). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
  61. Valle-Arroyo, F. (1989). Errores de lectura y escritura. Un modelo dual [Reading and spelling miscues: A dual route model]. Cognitiva, 2, 35–63Google Scholar
  62. Valle-Arroyo, F. (1996). Dual-route models in Spanish: Developmental and Neuropsychological data. In M. Carreiras, & N. Sebastián (Eds.), Language Processing in Spanish (pp. 89–118). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. E. Jiménez González

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations