Advertisement

Why Read Another Book on Human Development? Understanding Human Development Takes a Metatheory and Multiple Disciplines

  • Ursula M. Staudinger
  • Ulman Lindenberger
Chapter

Abstract

The description, explanation, and enhancement of development in individuals and groups of individuals are seen as important goals of scientific inquiry in most of the social, behavioral, and life sciences. At the same time, scholars working in these three research traditions increasingly agree that cultural and biological processes interact in the co-construction of human ontogeny. This broad and growing consensus provides a fertile common ground for scientific exchange. In the process, communication across research traditions and disciplines, while acknowledging necessary differences in terminology and methodology, needs to establish a common conceptual framework. Psychology, with its links to sociology, cultural anthropology, and history, on the one hand, and to cognitive neuroscience, evolutionary biology, and developmental biology, on the other, appears to be in a good position to mediate such a framework. The general goal of the present volume is to explore the tenability of this assertion.

Keywords

Human Development Successful Aging Multivariate Behavioral Research Dynamic Factor Analysis Lifespan Development 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bakes, P. B. (1987). Developmental psychology. In G. L. Maddox (Ed.), The encyclopedia of aging (pp. 170–175). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  2. Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52,366–380.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The models of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology, 28,121–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baltes, P. B., & Brim, O. G. J. (1978). Life-span development and behavior. (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.), Vol. 1, Theoretical models of human development (pp. 1029–1143). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  7. Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (Eds.). (1977). Lifespan developmental psychology: An introduction to research methods. Montery, CA: Brooks Cole (reprinted Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1988).Google Scholar
  8. Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span developmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 65–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M, & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Lifespan psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50,471–507.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blossfeld, H.-R, Hamerle, A., & Mayer, K. U. (1991). Event-history models in social mobility research. In D. Magnusson, L. R. Berman, G. Rudinger, & B. Törrestad (Eds.), Problems and methods in longitudinal research (pp. 212–235). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boker, S. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2002). A method for modeling the intrinsic dynamics of intraindividual variability: Recovering the parameters of simulated oscillators in multi-wave data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 37, 127–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.), Vol. 3, Social emotional, and personality development (pp. 311–387). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  13. Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  14. Elbert, T., Pantev, C, Wienbruch, C, Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–99.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Freund, A., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). The role of selection, optimization, and compensation in successful aging. In J. Brandtstadter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the lifespan (pp. 401–434). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & The ABC Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Goulet, L. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1970). Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Horn, J. L. (1989). Models of intelligence. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Intelligence: Measurement, theory, and public policy (pp. 29–73). Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  22. Kirkwood, T. B. L. (1977). Evolution of aging. Nature, 270, 301–304.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kirkwood, T. B. L. (1997). The origins of human aging. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, B, 352, 1765–1772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Li, S.-C. (in press). Biocultural orchestration of cognition: Concerted biological and cultural influences in turning the mind’s epigenesis across life span. Psychological BulletinGoogle Scholar
  26. Lindenberger, U. (2001). Lifespan theories of cognitive development. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 8848–8854). Oxford, England: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lindenberger, U. (2000). Intellectual Development across the Life Span: Overview and Selected Empirical Evidence. Psychologische Rundschau, 51,135–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Lifespan Developmental Psychology (Lifespan Psychology): Honoring Johann Nicolaus Tetens (1736–1807) Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 207,299–323.Google Scholar
  29. Magnusson, D., & Stattin, H. (1998). Person-context interaction theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Theoretical models of human development, Vol. 1, Handbook of child psychology (pp. 685–759). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  30. Molenaar, P. C. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1998). A comparison of pseudo-maximum likelihood and asymptotically distribution-free dynamic factor analysis parameter estimation in fitting covariance-structure models to block-Toeplitz matrices representing single-subject multivariate time series. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 313–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nesselroade, J. R., & Bartsch, T. W. (1977). Multivariate experimental perspectives on the construct validity of the trait-state distinction. In R. B. Cattell & R. M. Dreger (Eds.), Handbook of modern personality theory. New York: Hemisphere.Google Scholar
  32. Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American Psychologist, 31, 689–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rutter, M. (1996). Transitions and turning points in developmental psychopathology: As applied to the age span between childhood and mid-adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 603–626.Google Scholar
  34. Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 2, Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 801–847). New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
  35. Staudinger, U. M, & Pasupathi, M. (2000). Life-span perspectives on self, personality and social cognition. In T. Salthouse & F. Craik (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and aging (pp. 633–688). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  36. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ. A triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Tetens, J. N. (1777). Philosophical Trials on Human Nature and Its Development. Leipzig: Weidmanns Erben & Reich.Google Scholar
  38. Thomae, H. (1959). Developmental Psychology. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  39. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural selection and the evolution of senescence. Evolution, 11, 398–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ursula M. Staudinger
    • 1
  • Ulman Lindenberger
    • 2
  1. 1.Dresden UniversityDresdenGermany
  2. 2.Saarland UniversitySaarbrückenGermany

Personalised recommendations