Advertisement

Language Parallels in New World Primates

Chapter

Abstract

New World monkeys are less well known and less frequently studied than Old World monkeys and apes, yet they have value as models for speech and language. All New World primates are arboreal living in dense forests. This has led to a reliance on vocal communication rather than the visual signaling most common in Old World primates and apes. As a result New World primates have evolved complex vocal repertoires that may share more parallels with human speech than other primates. Many of these species also live in small family groups akin to most human societies. I describe the rationale for studying speech and language parallels in New World primates and present some of the methods used in research. I then discuss several areas of potential parallels including vocal complexity, categorical responses to calls, signals that refer to external objects or events, syntax and rudimentary grammar, developmental processes including babbling, dialects, and vocal control. I conclude with a brief discussion of cognitive abilities in these species some of which have important parallels to human cognition. Although New World primates have not often been subjects of research relating to speech and language disorders, they provide much potential for understanding mechanisms and developmental and functional aspects of speech and language.

Keywords

New World primates Vocal complexity Referential signals Syntax Babbling Vocal control Cognition 

References

  1. 1.
    Slocombe K (2012) Have we underestimated great ape vocal capacities? In: Gibson KR, Tallerman M (eds) Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Marler P (1970) Bird song and speech development: could there be parallels? Am Sci 58:669–673PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Castro NA, Snowdon CT (2000) Development of vocal responses in infant cotton-top tamarins. Behaviour 137:629–646CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Bauers KA, Snowdon CT (1990) Discrimination of chirp vocalizations in the cotton-top tamarin. Am J Primatol 21:53–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Snowdon CT, Cleveland J (1980) Individual recognition of contact calls in pygmy marmosets. Anim Behav 28:717–727CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Cleveland J, Snowdon CT (1982) The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus). Z Tierpsychol 58:231–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Pola YV, Snowdon CT (1975) The vocalizations of pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea). Anim Behav 23:826–842CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Snowdon CT, Hodun A (1981) Acoustic adaptations in pygmy marmoset contact calls: locational cues vary with distance between conspecifics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 9:295–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    De la Torre S, Snowdon CT (2002) Environmental correlates of vocal communication of wild pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea. Anim Behav 63:847–856CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Snowdon CT, Pola YV (1978) Interspecific and intraspecific responses to synthesized pygmy marmoset vocalizations. Anim Behav 26:192–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Snowdon CT (1987) A naturalistic view of categorical perception. In: Harnad S (ed) Categorical perception. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 332–354Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2003) Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302:1234–1236CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and urgency. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 268:2315–2324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Owings DH, Hennessy DF (1984) The importance of variation in sciurid visual and vocal communication. In: Murie JA, Michener GR (eds) The biology of ground dwelling squirrels. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp 169–200Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Elowson AM, Tannenbaum PL, Snowdon CT (1991) Food associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins. Anim Behav 42:931–937CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Benz JJ, Leger DW, French A (1992) Relations between food preferences and food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). J Comp Psychol 106:142–149CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Benz JJ (1993) Food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins: design features for representational communication. Anim Behav 45:443–455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    DiBitetti MS (2003) Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour 140:565–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    DiBitetti MS (2005) Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Anim Behav 69:911–919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Gros-Louis J (2004) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Anim Behav 67:431–440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Gros-Louis J (2006) Acoustic analysis and contextual description of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Int J Primatol 27:273–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Pollick AS, Gouzoules H, DeWaal FBM (2005) Audience effects on food calls in captive brown capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 70:1273–1280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Roush RS, Snowdon CT (2000) Quality, quantity, distribution and audience effects on food-calling in cotton-top tamarins. Ethology 106:673–690CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Chapman CA, Lefebvre L (1990) Manipulating foraging group size: spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees. Anim Behav 39:891–896CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Digweed SM, Fedigan LM, Rendell D (2005) Variable specificity in the anti-predator vocalizations and behaviour of the white-faced capuchin, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour 142:997–1022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Fichtel C, Perry S, Gros-Louis J (2005) Alarm calls of white-faced capuchin monkeys: an acoustic analysis. Anim Behav 70:165–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Wheeler BC (2010) Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:989–1000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Casar C, Byrne RW, Hoppitt W, Young RJ, Zuberbuhler K (2012) Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Anim Behav 84:405–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax)—evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Robinson JG (1979) An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey (Callicebus moloch). Z Tierpsychol 1979(49):381–405Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    McConnell PB, Snowdon CT (1986) Vocal interactions between unfamiliar groups of captive cotton top tamarins. Behaviour 97:273–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL (1996) Statistical learning in by 8-month old infants. Science 274:1926–1928CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Hauser MD, Newport EL, Aslin RN (2001) Segmentation of the speech steam in a non-human primate: statistical learning in cotton-top tamarins. Cognition 78:B53–B64CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Newport EL, Hauser MD, Spaepen G, Aslin RN (2004) Learning at a distance: statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in a non-human primate. Cogn Psychol 49:85–117CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Saffran J, Hauser M, Seibel R, Kapfhamer J, Tsao F, Cushman F (2008) Grammatical pattern learning by human infants and cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Cognition 107:479–500CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Snowdon CT, Cleveland J (1984) “Conversations” among pygmy marmosets. Am J Primatol 7:15–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Winter P, Hanley P, Ploog D, Schott D (1973) Ontogeny of squirrel monkey vocalizations under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour 47:230–239CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Herzog M, Hopf S (1983) Effects of species-specific vocalizations on the behaviour of surrogate reared squirrel monkeys. Behaviour 86:197–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Herzog M, Hopf S (1984) Behavioral responses to species-specific warning calls in infant squirrel monkeys reared in social isolation. Am J Primatol 7:99–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    McCowan B, Franceschini NV, Vicino GA (2001) Age differences and developmental trends in alarm peep responses by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Am J Primatol 53:19–31CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Lieblich AK, Symmes D, Newman JP, Shapiro M (1980) Development of the isolation peep in laboratory bred squirrel monkeys. Anim Behav 28:1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Oller DK (2000) The emergence of the speech capacity. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Elowson AM, Snowdon CT, Lazaro-Perea C (1998) Infant “babbling” in a nonhuman primate: complex sequences of vocal behavior. Behaviour 135:643–664CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Snowdon CT, Elowson AM (2001) “Babbling” in pygmy marmosets: development after infancy. Behaviour 138:1235–1248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Goldstein MH, King AP, West ME (2003) Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:8030–8035CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. 46.
    Goldstein MH, Schwade JA, Briesch J, Syal S (2010) Learning while babbling: prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations signal a readiness to learn. Infancy 15:362–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. 47.
    Roush RS, Snowdon CT (1994) Ontogeny of food associated calls in cotton-top tamarins. Anim Behav 47:263–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. 48.
    Roush RS, Snowdon CT (1999) The effects of social status on food-associated calling behavior in captive cotton-top tamarins. Anim Behav 58:1299–1305CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Mitani JC, Brandt K (1994) Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-range distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology 96:322–352Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Mitani JC, Hasegawa T, Gros-Louis J, Marler P, Byrne R (1992) Dialects in wild chimpanzees? Am J Primatol 27:233–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Mitani JC, Hunley KL, Murdoch ME (1999) Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. Am J Primatol 47:133–151CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Crockford C, Herbinger L, Vigilant L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. 53.
    Elowson AM, Snowdon CT (1994) Pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea) modify vocal structure in response to changed social environments. Anim Behav 47:1267–1277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Snowdon CT, Elowson AM (1999) Pygmy marmosets alter call structure when paired. Ethology 105:893–908CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. 55.
    Rukstalis M, Fite JE, French JA (2003) Social change affects vocal structure in a Callitrichid primate (Callithrix kuhlii). Ethology 109:327–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    De la Torre S, Snowdon CT (2009) Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. Am J Primatol 71:333–342CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. 57.
    Yepez P, de la Torre S, Snowdon CT (2005) Inter-population differences in exudate feeding of pygmy marmosets in Ecuadorian Amazonia. Am J Primatol 66:145–158CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Snowdon CT, Boe CY (2003) Social communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins. J Comp Psychol 117:142–148CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. 59.
    Visalberghi E, Addessi E (2000) Response to changes in food palatability in tufted capuchin monkeys Cebus apella. Anim Behav 59:231–238CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. 60.
    Hayes SL, Snowdon CT (1990) Predator recognition in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Am J Primatol 20:283–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Campbell MW, Snowdon CT (2009) Can auditory playback condition predator mobbing in captive-reared cotton-top tamarins? Int J Primatol 30:93–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. 62.
    Campbell MW, Snowdon CT (2007) Vocal response of captive-reared Saguinus oedipus during mobbing. Int J Primatol 28:257–270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. 63.
    Brumm H, Viss K, Köllmer I, Todt D (2004) Acoustic communication in noise: regulation of call characteristics in a New World monkey. J Exp Biol 207:443–448CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. 64.
    Egnor SER, Hauser MD (2006) Noise-induced vocal modulation in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Am J Primatol 68:1183–1190CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Miller CT, Flusberg D, Hauser MD (2003) Interruptibility of long call production in tamarins: implications for vocal development. J Exp Biol 206:2629–2639CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. 66.
    Egnor SER, Iguina CG (2006) Hauser MD perturbation of auditory feedback causes systematic perturbation in vocal structure in cotton-top tamarins. J Exp Biol 209:3652–3663CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Burkart JM, van Schaik C (2010) Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates. Anim Cogn 13:1–19CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Moscovice LR, Snowdon CT (2006) The role of social context and individual experience in novel task acquisition in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Behav 71:933–943CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Voelkl B, Huber L (2000) True imitation in marmosets. Anim Behav 60:195–202CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. 70.
    Joyce SM, Snowdon CT (2007) Developmental changes in food transfers in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Am J Primatol 69:955–965CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. 71.
    Humle T, Snowdon CT (2008) Socially biased learning in the acquisition of a complex foraging task in juvenile cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Behav 75:267–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Dillis C, Humle T, Snowdon CT (2010) Socially biased learning among adult cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Am J Primatol 72:287–295PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. 73.
    Rapaport LG, Brown GR (2008) Social influences on foraging behavior: in young nonhuman primates: learning what, where, and how to eat. Evol Anthropol 17:189–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Humle T, Snowdon CT, Matsuzawa T (2009) Social influences on the acquisition of ant dipping among the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Anim Cogn 12:S37–S48CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. 75.
    Cronin KA, Snowdon CT (2008) The effects of unequal reward distributions on cooperative problem solving by cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Anim Behav 75:245–257CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Mendres KA, de Waal FBM (2000) Capuchins do cooperate: the advantage of an intuitive task. Anim Behav 60:523–529CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. 77.
    de Waal FBM, Berger ML (2000) Payment for labour in monkeys. Nature 404:563CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Mascaro J, Schapiro SJ (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature 437:1357–1359CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. 79.
    Burkart JM, Fehr E, Efferson C, van Schaik CP (2007) Other-regarding preferences in a nonhuman primate: common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:19762–19766CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. 80.
    Cronin KA, Schroeder KK, Snowdon CT (2010) Prosocial behaviour emerges independent of reciprocity in cottontop tamarins. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 277:3845–3851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. 81.
    Whitten AJ, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. 82.
    Perry S, Baker M, Fedigan L, Gros-Louis J, Jack K, MacKinnon KC, Manson JH, Panger M, Pyle K, Rose L (2003) Social conventions in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys Evidence for traditions in a neotropical primate. Curr Anthropol 44:241–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. 83.
    Otoni EB, Izar P (2008) Capuchin monkey tool use: overview and implications. Evol Anthropol 17:171–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. 84.
    Visalberghi E, Fragaszy D, Ottoni EB, Izar P, Gomes de Oliveira M, Andrade FRD. Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm nuts. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2007;132:426–44.Google Scholar
  85. 85.
    Miller C, Dimauro A, Pistorio A, Hendry S, Wang X (2010) Vocalization induced cFos expression in marmoset cortex. Front Integr Neurosci 4:128CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. 86.
    Johnson LA, Della Santina CC, Wang X (2012) Temporal bone characterization and cochlear implant feasibility in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Hear Res 290:37–44CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. 87.
    Ferris CF, Snowdon CT, King JA, Sullivan JM Jr, Ziegler TE, Ludwig R, Schultz-Darken N, Wu Z, Olson DP, Tannenbaum PL, Einspanier A, Vaughn JT, Duong TQ (2004) Imaging neural pathways associated with stimuli for sexual arousal in nonhuman primates. J Magn Reson Imaging 19:168–175CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WisconsinMadisonUSA

Personalised recommendations