Allen, G. I., & Tsukahara, N. (1974). Cerebrocerebellar communication systems. Physiological Reviews, 54(4), 957–1006.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bennett, M., & Hacker, P. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Google Scholar
Blackmore, S. (2007). Mind over matter? Many philosophers and scientists have argued that free will is an illusion. Unlike all of them, Benjamin Libet found a way to test it. The Guardian Unlimited. Retrieved from http://commentisfree.guardian.co.uk/sue_blackmore/.
Blinowska, K., Kus, R., Kaminski, M., & Janiszewska, J. (2010). Transmission of brain activity during cognitive task. Brain Topography, 23(2), 205–213.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Boly, M., Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Hampshire, A., Bor, D., Moonen, G., et al. (2007). When thoughts become action: An fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity in non-communicative brain injured patients. NeuroImage, 36(3), 979–992. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.047.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (2006). Patricia and Paul Churchland in Conversations on consciousness: What the best minds think about the brain, free will and what it means to be human. (S Blackmore, Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Coles, M. G. (1989). Modern mind-brain reading: Psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. Psychophysiology, 26(3), 251–269.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
DeLong, M. R., Alexander, G. E., Mitchell, S. J., & Richardson, R. T. (1986). The contribution of basal ganglia to limb control. Progress in Brain Research, 64, 161–174. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63411-1.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 308(1135), 67–78. doi:10.1098/rstb.1985.0010.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1998). The lateralized readiness potential as an on-line measure of central response activation processes. Behavior Research Methods, 30, 146–156.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1481–1489. doi:10.1038/nn1579.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (1998). The neuronal platonist. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 706–717.
Google Scholar
Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: Single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.□: 1991), 19(6), 1239–1255. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn181.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 718–726. http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=115977.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Giménez-Amaya, J. M. (2011). A better understanding of freedom. An interdisciplinary approach to neuroscience and philosophy. In J. Sanguineti, A. Acerbi, & J. Lombo (Eds.), Moral behavior and free will: A neurobiological and philosophical approach (pp. 47–60). Morolo: IF Press.
Google Scholar
Giménez-Amaya, J. M., & Murillo, J. I. (2009). Neurociencia y libertad: Una aproximación interdisciplinar. Scripta Theologica, 41, 13–46.
Google Scholar
Graybiel, A. M. (2008). Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 359–387. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112851.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2011). Does brain science change our view of free will? In R. Swinburne (Ed.), Free will and modern science (pp. 7–24). New York: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Haggard, P., & Eimer, M. (1999). On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Experimental Brain Research, 126(1), 128–133.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hirsh, R. (1974). The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. Behavioral Biology, 12(4), 421–444.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Keller, I., & Heckhausen, H. (1990). Readiness potentials preceding spontaneous motor acts: Voluntary vs. involuntary control. Electroencephalography and Clinica Neurophysiology, 76(4), 351–361.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kilner, J. M., Vargas, C., Duval, S., Blakemore, S.-J., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nature Neuroscience, 7(12), 1299–1301. doi:10.1038/nn1355.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Changes in the brain potential in voluntary movements and passive movements in man: Readiness potential and reafferent potentials. Pflügers Archiv für die Gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere, 284, 1.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1999). Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 47–57.
Google Scholar
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Brain, 106, 623–642.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology: CB, 20(8), 750–756. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Murillo, J. I., & Giménez-Amaya, J. M. (2008). Tiempo, conciencia y libertad: consideraciones en torno a los experimentos de B. Libet y colaboradores. Acta Philosophica, 11(17), 291–306.
Google Scholar
Murray, G. K., Clark, L., Corlett, P. R., Blackwell, A. D., Cools, R., Jones, P. B., et al. (2008). Incentive motivation in first-episode psychosis: A behavioural study. BMC Psychiatry, 8(8), 34. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-8-34.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Nieuwenhuis, S., Voogd, J., & Van Huijzen, C. (2008). The human central nervous system. Heidelberg: Springer.
Google Scholar
Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science (New York, N.Y.), 313(5792), 1402. doi:10.1126/science.1130197.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Polo, L. (1996). Tener y dar. Sobre la existencia cristiana (pp. 103–135). Pamplona: EUNSA.
Google Scholar
Polo, L., & Llano, C. (1997). Los hábitos. Antropología de la acción directiva (pp. 103–112). Madrid: Unidad Editorial.
Google Scholar
Raffone, A., & Pantani, M. (2010). A global workspace model for phenomenal and access consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 580–596. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.013.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rettew, D. C., Swedo, S. E., Leonard, H. L., Lenane, M. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1992). Obsessions and compulsions across time in 79 children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(6), 1050–1056.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131–141.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20(1), 11–21.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 163–196.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Seger, C. A., & Spiering, B. J. (2011). A critical review of habit learning and the basal ganglia. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5, 66. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2011.00066.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Smith, K. (2011). Neuroscience vs philosophy: Taking aim at free will. Nature, 477(7362), 23–25.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(5), 543–545. doi:10.1038/nn.2112.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515–13522.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253(5026), 1380–1386.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J., & Balleine, B. W. (2004). Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(1), 181–189. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x/abstract.
Google Scholar
Zhu, J. (2003). Reclaiming volition. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19(11), 61–77.
Google Scholar