Skip to main content

Social Learning Theory: A Contextualist Account of Cognitive Functioning

  • Chapter

Part of the Springer Series in Cognitive Development book series (2116)

Abstract

Social learning theory grew out of the efforts of Bandura and Walters (1959, 1963) to explain how children acquired information and behavior by observing people in natural settings. Initially they investigated youngsters’ simple imitation of common responses, such as aggression, by a model. Favorable results of this research prompted study of more complex classes of social learning, such as the development of emotional reactions (attraction and avoidance), cognitive and linguistic rules, self-regulating responses, personal standards, expectations, and self-efficacy judgments. This social interactionist approach to development revealed a distinctive but widely underestimated feature of children’s knowledge: At all levels of complexity, it remained highly dependent on the social environmental context from which it sprang. This property of thought also became evident to other theorists as they began to study cognitive functioning in naturalistic settings. Several of these theorists have discussed the implications of their research on the basis of a general epistemology termed “contextualism.”

Keywords

  • Social Learning
  • Rule Learning
  • Language Acquisition
  • Outcome Expectation
  • Apply Behavior Analysis

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4613-9490-7_1
  • Chapter length: 50 pages
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
eBook
USD   59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • ISBN: 978-1-4613-9490-7
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
Softcover Book
USD   74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adorno, T. W. Sociology and psychology. New Left Review, November-December 1967, 67–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adorno, T. W. Introduction. In T. W. Adorno, H. Albert, R. Dahrendorf, J. Habermas, H. Pilot, & K. R. Popper (Eds.), The positivist dispute in German sociology. New York: Harper & Row, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, K. E., Henke, L. B., Harris, F. R., Baer, D. M., & Reynolds, N. J. Control of hyperactivity by social reinforcement of attending behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1967, 58, 231–237.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arem, C. A., & Zimmerman, B. J. Vicarious effects on the creative behavior on retarded and nonretarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1976, 81, 289–296.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Labouvie, G. V. Adult development of intellectual performance: Description, explanation, and modification. In D. Eisdorfer & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and aging. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 10). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Influence of a model’s reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 11, 587–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Psychological mo deling-Conflicting theories. Chicago: Atherton/Aldine, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Effecting change through participant modeling. In J. D. Krumboltz & C. E. Thoresen (Eds.), Counseling method. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Self efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 191–215. (a)

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1977. (b)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. On paradigms and recycled ideologies. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1978, 2, 79–103. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 344–358. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. The self and mechanisms of agency. In J. Suls (Ed.), Social psychological perspectives on the self. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 1982, 37, 122–147. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. The psychology of chance encounters and life paths. American Psychologist, 1982, 37, 747–755. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. The relative efficacy of desensitization and modeling approaches for inducing behavioral, affective, and attitudinal changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1969, 13, 173–199.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Some social determinants of self-monitoring reinforcement systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 449–455. (a)

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. Vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 16–23. (b)

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Harris, M. B. Modification of syntactic style. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 4, 341–352.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Jeffery, R. W. Role of symbolic coding and rehearsal processes in observational learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 26, 122–130.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Jeffery, R. W., & Bachicha, D. L. Analysis of memory codes and cumulative rehearsal in observational learning. Journal of Research in Personality, 1974, 7, 295–305.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Kupers, C. J. The transmission of patterns of self reinforcement through modeling. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 69, 1–9.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & McDonald, F. J. Influences of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children’s moral judgments. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274–281.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Perloff, B. Relative efficacy of self-monitored and externally imposed reinforcement systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 7, 111–116.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. Vicarious classical conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 54–62.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. Imitation of film mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 3–11.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. Cultivating competence, self efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1981, 41, 586–598.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Adolescent aggression. New York: Ronald Press, 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Walters, R. Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilin, H. Piaget and the new functionalism. Paper presented at the 11th symposium of the Jean Piaget Society, Philadelphia, May 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belcher, T. L. Modeling original divergent responses: An initial investigation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975, 67, 351–358.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bengtson, V. L. The social psychology of aging. New York: Bobbs-Merrill, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L., & Luckman, T. The social construction of reality. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. Children’s reasoning and thinking. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3d ed.). New York: Wiley, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berzonsky, M. D. Interdependence of Inhelder and Piaget’s model of logical thinking. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 4, 469–476.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S., & Baer, D. Behavioral analysis of child development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, A. H. The extinction of avoidance responses under curare. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1958, 51, 519–524.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. Talking, understanding, and thinking. In R. L. Schiefelbusch and L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives-Acquisition, retardation, and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. Language development review. In F. D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botwinick, J. Aging and behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, T. G. R. The visual world of infants. Scientific American, 1966, 215, 80–92.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, K. S. Situationism in psychology: An analysis and a critique. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 307–336.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. Cognitive development and concept learning: An interpretive review. Psychological Bulletin, 1977, 84, 919–939.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. Learning, research, and Piagetian theory. In L. S. Siegel & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Alternatives to Piaget. New York: Academic Press, 1978. (a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C.J. The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1978, 2, 173–213. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brofenbrenner, U. The ecology of human development. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., Smiley, S. S., Days, J. D. Townsend, M. A., & Lawton, S. C. Intrusion of a thematic idea in children’s comprehension and retention of stories. Child Development, 1977, 48, 1454–1466.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, I., Jr. Role of referent concreteness in the acquisition of passive sentence comprehension through abstract modeling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976, 22, 185–199.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, I., Jr., & Inouye, D. K. Learned helplessness through modeling: The role of perceived similarity in competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 900–908.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. Development of the first language in the human species. American Psychologist, 1913, 28, 97–106.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. B. Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck-Morss, S. Socio-economic bias in Piaget’s theory and its implication for cross-culture studies. Human Development, 1975, 18, 35–49.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. On conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 1975, 30, 1103–1126.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, W. R., Rosenthal, T. L., & Brysh, C. G. Social transmission of grammatical parameters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1972, 63, 589–596.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. Social cognition and life-span approaches to the study of cognitive development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, B. B., & Ericson, C. W. Cognitive factors in heart rate conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 64, 272–279.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. Knowledge structures and memory development. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. B., & Sherman, J. A. Teaching generative use of sentence answers to three forms of questions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1975, 8, 321–330.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Brownell, H. H. Judging up and down. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975, 1, 339–352.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coates, T. J., & Thoresen, C. E. Behavioral self-control and educational practice or do we really need self control? In D. Berliner (Ed.), Review of research in education. Washington, D.C.: American Educational Research Association, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Bruner, J. S. Cultural differences and inferences about psychological processes. American Psychologist, 1971, 26, 867–876.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & Sharp, D. The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Scribner, S. Culture and thought: A psychological introduction. New York: Wiley, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dannhauser, W. J. A review of the philosophy of moral development. (Moral stages and the idea of justice, Vol. 1: Essays on moral development by L. Kohlberg). New York Times Book Review, 1981, 8 (August 9), 11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Datan, N., & Ginsberg, L. H. (Eds.). Life span developmental psychology: Normative life crises. New York: Academic Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, H. R., & Lang, G. Children’s perceptions of their teachers’ feelings toward them related to self-perception, school achievement, and behavior. Journal of Experimental Education, 1960, 29, 107–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Debus, R. L. Effects of brief observation and model behavior on conceptual tempo of impulsive children. Developmental Psychology, 1970, 2, 22–32.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Debus, R. L. Observational learning of reflective strategies by impulsive children. Paper presented at the Congrès International de Psychologie, Paris 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N., & Earl, R. W. Analysis of exploratory, manipulatory, and curiosity behaviors. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 91–96.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dulaney, D. E. Awareness, rules, and propositional control: A confrontation with S-R behavior theory. In T. R. Dixon and D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanders, N. A. Teacher influence, pupil attitudes, and achievement. Cooperative Research Monograph, 1965 (No. 12). U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Office of Education, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, N.J.: Van Nostrand-Reinhold, 1963.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. Cognitive changes in adulthood. In P. B. Baltes & L. R. Goulet (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. Metamemory. In R. V. Vail & J. H. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. E. The construct validity of egocentrism. Psychological Bulletin, 1979, 86, 1169–1188.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. The basic writings of Sigmund Freud (F. A. Brill, Ed.). New York: Modern Library, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne, R. M. Contributions of learning to human development. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 177–191.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garbarino, J., & Brofenbrenner, U. The socialization of moral judgment and behavior in cross-cultural perspective. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 26, 309–320.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. Toward generative theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 1344–1360.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gerst, M. S. Symbolic coding operations in observational learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971, 19, 7–17.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gibb, J. C. The meaning of ecologically oriented inquiry in contemporary psychology. American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 127–140.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. An ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glick, J. Cognitive development in cross-cultural perspective. In F. D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glynn, E. L. Classroom applications of self determined reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 123–132.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodnow, J.J. The nature of intelligent behavior: Questions raised by cross-cultural studies. In L. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guess, D., Sailor, W., Rutherford, G., & Baer, D. M. An experimental analysis of linguistic development: The productive use of the plural morpheme. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 297–306.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. Knowledge and human interests. Boston: Beacon Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B., & Evans, R. C. Models and creativity. Psychological Reports, 1973, 33, 163–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B., & Fisher, J. L. Modeling and flexibility in problem solving. Psychological Reports, 1913, 33, 19–23.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B., & Hassemer, W. G. Some factors affecting the complexity of children’s sentences: The effects of modeling, age, sex, and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1972, 13, 447–455.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. B., & Siebel, C. E. Effects of sex, occupation, and confidence of model and sex and grade of subject on imitation of language behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 1976, 12, 89–90.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, R. W., Swanson, R., & Zimmerman, B. J. Inquiry response induction of preschool children through televised modeling. Developmental Psychology, 1974, 11, 523–524.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, R. W., Swanson, R., & Zimmerman, B. J. Training seriation responses in young children through televised modeling of hierarchically sequenced rule components. American Educational Research Journal, 1975, 12, 474–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, R. W., Zimmerman, B. J., Swanson, R., & Bergan, J. R. Televised cognitive skill instruction for Papago native American children. Tucson: Arizona Center for Education Research and Development, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, L., & Bloom, L. What, when, and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1979, 44 (6, Serial No. 181).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. Differential effects of incidental tasks on the organization of recall of a list of highly associated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 82, 412–481.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. The principles of psychology. New York: Holt, 1890.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. J. Remember that old theory of memory? Well, forget it! American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 785–795.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A., Baron, A., & Kopp, R. E. Some effects of instructions on human operant behavior. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1966, 1, 243–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. H. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistomology. New York: Academic Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, M. Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, Ill: Dorsey Press, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., & Chandler, M. J. Cognitive development: Idealism vs. contextualism. In P. B. Baltes (Ed.), Life span development and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamal, P. A. Imitative learning of information-processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1971, 12, 223–227.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P. R., Moss, I. L., & Miller, S. M. Information processing in children as a function of adult model, stimulus display, school grade, and sex. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1969, 60, 188–193.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M. Moral development: A theoretical and empirical analysis. In G. J. Whitehurst & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Functions of language and cognition. New York: Academic Press, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. Cognitive and emotional components of text anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 1967, 20, 975–978.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M., Odom, R. D., Hill, J. H., & Huff, R. L. The effects of age and rule familiarity on the production of modeled language constructions. Developmental Psychology, 1969, 1, 108–112.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M., & Swenson, S. A. Abstraction, inferences, and the process of imitative learning. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 500–504. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Liebert, R. M., & Swenson, S. A. Association and abstraction as mechanisms of imitative learning. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 4, 289–294. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O. R. Geriatric behavioral prosthetics. In R. Kastenbaum (Ed.), New thoughts on old age. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukacs, G. History and class consciousness. New York: Random House, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNamara, J. Cognitive basis of language learning in infants. Psychological Review, 1972, 7, 195–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M. J. Cognition and behavior modification. Cambridge, Mass.: Ballinger, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. Organization and memory. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, M. E., & Van Wagenen, R. K. Alteration of joint mother-child linguistic styles, involving procedures of extension, elaboration and reinforcement. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, April 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. Testing for competence rather than “intelligence.” American Psychologist, 1973, 25, 1–14.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. W., & Lowell, E. L. The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1953.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. The acquisition of language: The study of developmental psycholinguistics. New York: Harper & Row, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven: Institute of Human Relations, Yale University Press, 1941.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 252–283.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. The interaction of person and situation. In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the cross roads: Current issues in interactional psychology. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. On the interface of cognition and personality: Beyond the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 740–754.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., & Liebert, R. M. Effects of discrepancies between observed and imposed reward criteria on their acquisition and transmission. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 5, 45–53.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. Processes of language teaching and language learning in the interaction of mother-child dyads. Child Development, 1976, 47, 1064–1078.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Morgulas, S. The effect of information about sentence referents on children’s observational learning of a syntactic rule. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Graduate School of the City University of New York, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogulas, S., & Zimmerman, B. J. The role of comprehension in children’s observational learning of a syntactic rule. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1979, 28, 455–468.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, L. W., & Liebert, R. M. Relationship of cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety to physiology arousal and academic performance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1970, 35, 332–337.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, F. B. Acquisition of conservation through social interaction. Developmental Pyschology, 1972, 6, 1–6.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mussen, P. H. Foreward. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Strategies and techniques of child study. New York: Academic Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, N. A., & Perlmutter, M. Memory in the years two to five. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Applet on-Century-Crofts, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. Cognition and reality. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. Some evidence for the cognitive primacy of categorization and its functional basis. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 1973, 19, 21–39. (a)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1973, 38(1–2, Serial No. 149). (b)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, 267–285.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. How children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language: A preliminary report. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R., Schaie, K. W., & Baltes, P. B. Ontogenetic and generational components of structural and quantitative change in adult cognitive behavior. Journal of Gerontology, 1972, 27, 222–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neugarten, B. L., & Datan, N. Sociological perspectives on the life cycle. In P. B. Baltes & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Personality and socialization. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odom, R. D., Liebert, R. M., & Hill, J. H. The effects of modeling cues, rewards, and attention set on the properties of grammatical and ungrammatical syntactic constructions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1968, 6, 131–140.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pepper, S. C. World hypotheses. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, J. R. Syntax and vocabulary of mother’s speech to young children. Child Development, 1913, 44, 182–185.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, Ill: Free Press, 1948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The psychology of intelligence. New York: Harcourt and Brace, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press, 1952.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, trans.). New York: Basic Books, 1954.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology. New York: Wiley, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development, 1912, 15, 1–12.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Proshansky, H. M. Environmental psychology and the real world. Psychologist, 1976, 31, 303–310.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J., & Hodgson, R. I. Synchrony and desynchrony in fear and avoidance. Behavior Research and Therapy, 1974, 12, 311–318.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Ridberg, E. H., Parke, R. D., & Hetherington, E. M. Modification of impulsive and reflective cognitive style through observation of film-mediated models. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 185–190.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, K. F. Adult life crises: A dialectical interpretation of development. In N. Datan and L. H. Ginsburg (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, K. F. From traits and equilibrium toward developmental dialectics. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol.23). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, M. E., & Schultz, L. J. The effects of extraneous response requirements on learning by performers and observers. Psychonomic Science, 1961, 8, 51–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., Alford, G. S., & Rasp, L. M. Concept attainment, generalization, and retention through observation and verbal coding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1972, 13, 183–194. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Bandura, A. Psychological modeling: Theory and practice. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergan (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Carroll, W. R. Factors in vicarious modification of complex grammatical parameters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1972, 63, 174–178.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Whitebook, J. S. Incentives versus instructions in transmitting grammatical parameters with experimenter as model. Behavior Research and Therapy, 1970, 5, 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., Zimmerman, B. J., & Durning, K. Observationally-induced changes in children’s interrogative classes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 681–688.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B.J. Instructional specificity and outcome expectation in observationally induced question formulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1972, 63, 500–504. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B. J. Modeling by exemplification and instruction in training conservation. Developmental Psychology, 1972, 6, 392–401. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B.J. Organization, observation and guided practice in concept attainment and generalization. Child Development, 1913, 44, 606–613.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B. J. Organization and stability of transfer in vicarious concept attainment. Child Development, 1976, 47, 110–117.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, T. L., & Zimmerman, B. J. Social learning and cognition. New York: Academic Press, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. Nonconservation of area: Do children succeed where adults fail? Developmental Psychology, 1976, 12, 467–468.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. Television is “easy” and print is “tough”: The differential investment of mental effort in learning as a function of perceptions and attributions. Journal of Educational Psychology, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. Psychology and the American ideal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1911, 35, 767–782.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. Scientific paradigms and social values: Wanted — A scientific revolution. Journal of Personality and Social Personality, 1978, 36, 1332–1343.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. Cognitive psychology as ideology. American Psychologist, 1981, 36, 730–743.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W., Labouvie, G. V., & Buech, B. U. Generational and cohort-specific differences in adult cognitive functioning: A fourteen-year study of independent samples. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 9, 151–166.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W., & Labouvie-Vief, G. Generational versus ontogenetic components of change in adult cognitive behavior: A fourteen-year cross-sequential study. Developmental Psychology, 1914, 10, 305–320.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W., & Strother, C. R. A cross-sectional study of age changes in cognitive behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 70, 671–680. (a)

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W., & Strother, C. R. The effects of time and cohort differences on the interpretation of age changes in cognitive behavior. Multivariate Behavior Research, 1968, 3, 259–294. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Schumaker, J., & Sherman, J. A. Training generative verb usage by imitation and reinforcement procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1970, 3, 273–287.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. Modeling and attribution effects on children’s achievement: A self-efficacy analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1981, 73, 93–105.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. The development of communication skill modification in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1973, 38(5, Serial No. 152).

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children, II: External reinforcement of conservation of weight and operations of addition and subtraction. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 2, 71–84. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 3, 153–155. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Bandura’s theory of self-efficacy: A set of common sense theorems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1978, 19, 1–14.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. Mother’s speech to children learning language. Child Development, 1972, 43, 549–565.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Strohner, H., & Nelson, K. The young child’s development of sentence comprehension: Influence of event probability, non-verbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. Child Development, 1914, 45, 189–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taub, E., Bacon, R. C., & Berman, A. J. Acquisition of a trace-conditioned avoidance response after deafferentation of the responding limb. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1965, 59, 275–279.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thoresen, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. Behavioral self-control. New York: Holt, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. Educational psychology, Vol. 1: The original nature of man. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobias, S. Achievement treatment interactions. Review of Educational Research, 1916, 46, 61–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. Subjective organization in free recall of related words. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 344–354.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasta, R., & Liebert, R. M. Auditory discrimination of novel prepositional constructions as a function of age and syntactic background. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 9, 79–82.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D. A. Memories of Morocco: The influence of age, schooling, and environment on memory. Cognitive Psychology, 1978, 10, 1–28.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, R. I. The great psychologists. Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weir, M. W. Developmental changes in problem solving strategies. Psychological Review, 1964, 71, 473–490.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., Ironsmith, M., & Goldfein, M. Selective imitation of the passive construction through modeling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1914, 17, 288–302.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Novak, G. Modeling, imitation training, and the acquisition of sentence phrase. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1973, 16, 332–345.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Willems, E. P. Behavioral technology and behavioral ecology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7, 151–166.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. Un essai d Apprentissage dans le domaine de la conservation due nombre. Estudes d’Epistémologie Génétique, 1959, 9, 125–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F., & Lowe, R. C. An experimental analysis of the conservation of number. Child Development, 1962, 33, 153–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yussen, S. R. Determinants of visual attention and recall in observational learning by preschoolers and second graders. Developmental Psychology, 1974, 10, 93–100.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B.J. Modification of young children’s grouping strategies: Effects of modeling, verbalization, incentive, and age. Child Development, 1974, 45, 1032–1041.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. Modeling. In H. Horn & P. Robinson (Eds.), Psychological processes in children’s early education. New York: Academic Press, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. A social learning explanation for age-related changes in children’s conceptual behavior. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1978, 3, 11–19.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. Concepts and classification. In G. J. Whitehurst & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Functions of language and cognition. New York: Academic Press, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. Operativity: A critic’s view of the construct and related research. Paper presented at the sixth biennial meeting of the Southeastern Conference on Human Development, Alexandria, Va., April 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B.J. Social learning theory and cognitive constructivism. In I. E. Sigel, D. M. Brodzinsky, & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), New directions in Piagetian theory and practice. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Bell, J. A. Observer verbalization and abstraction in vicarious rule learning, generalization, and retention. Developmental Psychology, 1972, 7, 227–231.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Blom, D. E. Cognitive conflict and learning: An empirical test of the construct. Development Review, 1983, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Dialessi, F. Modeling influences on children’s creative behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1973, 65, 127–134.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Jaffe, A. Teaching through demonstration: The effects of structuring, imitation, and age. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1977, 69, 773–778.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Kinsler, K. The effects of exposure to a punished model and verbal prohibitions in children’s toy play. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1979, 71, 388–395.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Kleefeld, C. F. Toward a theory of teaching: A social learning view. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1977, 2, 158–171.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Koussa, R. Sex factors in children’s observational learning of value judgments of toys. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 1975, 1, 121–133.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Koussa, R. Social influences on children’s toy preferences: Effects of model rewardingness and affect. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1979, 4, 55–66.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Lanaro, P. Acquiring and retaining conservation of length through modeling and reversibility cues. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1974, 20, 145–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Pike, E. O. Effects of modeling and reinforcement on the acquisition and generalization of question asking behavior. Child Development, 1972, 45, 892–907.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Ringle, J. Effects of model persistence and statements of confidence on children’s self efficacy and problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1981, 75, 485–493.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Rosenthal, T. L. Concept attainment, transfer, and retention through observation and rule provision. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1972, 14, 139–150. (a)

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Rosenthal, T. L. Observation repetition and ethnic background in concept attainment and generalization. Child Development, 1972, 43, 605–613. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Rosenthal, T. L. Conserving and retaining equalities and inequalities through observation and correction. Developmental Psychology, 1974, 10, 260–268. (a)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Rosenthal, T. L. Observational learning of rule governed behavior by children. Psychological Bulletin, 1974, 81, 29–42. (b)

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Whitehurst, G. J. Structure and function: A comparison of two views of the development of language and cognition. In G. J. Whitehurst and B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), The functions of language and cognition. New York: Academic Press, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 1983 Springer-Verlag, New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zimmerman, B.J. (1983). Social Learning Theory: A Contextualist Account of Cognitive Functioning. In: Brainerd, C.J. (eds) Recent Advances in Cognitive-Developmental Theory. Springer Series in Cognitive Development. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9490-7_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9490-7_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9492-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9490-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive