Resistance to Interference: Developmental Changes in a Basic Processing Mechanism

  • Frank N. Dempster

Abstract

Historically, theories of cognitive development have focused on two general processes: knowledge accumulation and information activation. Central to Piaget’s grand design, for example, is the notion that cognition is rule-governed (Reyna & Brainerd, 1991). He frequently stressed the importance of rule-based “operations” that, as they are acquired, progressively enlarge the child’s intellectual repertoire. Likewise, many more recent developments inspired by the rise of information-processing theories have explained age-related improvements in performance on the grounds that individuals acquire and implement more powerful rules, skills, or strategies (such as rehearsal, elaboration, and imagery) as they grow older (e.g., Fischer, 1980; Moely, 1977; Siegler, 1984). Although there are many points of divergence between Piagetian-type rules and information-processing rules, both traditions have nurtured the view that intellectual development is, in considerable part, a product of the accumulation of certain forms of knowledge.

Keywords

Selective Information Schizophrenia Resis Gall Sorting 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Albert, M.S., & Kaplan, E. (1980). Organic implications of neuropsychological deficits in the elderly. In L.W. Poon, J.L. Fozard, L.S. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L.W. Thompson (Eds.), New directions in memory and aging (pp. 403–432). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Anooshian, L.J., & McCulloch, R.A. (1979). Developmental changes in dichotic listening with categorized word lists. Developmental Psychology, 15, 280–287.Google Scholar
  3. Baillargeon, R., DeVos, T., & Graber, M. (1989). Location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task: Further evidence. Cognitive Development, 4, 345–367.Google Scholar
  4. Beech, A., Powell, T., McWilliams, J., & Claridge, G. (1989). Evidence of reduced “cognitive inhibition” in schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 110–116.Google Scholar
  5. Berg, E.A. (1948). A simple objective test for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 378–379.Google Scholar
  6. Bianchi, L. (1895). The function of the frontal lobes. Brain, 18, 497–530.Google Scholar
  7. Bjork E.L., & Cummings, E.M. (1984). Infant search errors: Stage of concept development or stage of memory development. Memory & Cognition, 12, 1–19.Google Scholar
  8. Bjork, R.A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H.L. Roediger, III & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Bjorklund, D.F. (1985). The role of conceptual knowledge in the development of organization in children’s memory. In C.J. Brained & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development: Progress in cognitive development research (pp. 103–142). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  10. Bjorklund, D.F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.Google Scholar
  11. Bjorklund, D.F., & Harnishfeger, K.K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.Google Scholar
  12. Bruner, J.S. (1966). On the conservation of liquids. In J.S. Bruner, R.R. Olver, & P.M. Greenfield, et al., (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 183–207). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
  13. Case, R. (1974). Structures and strictures: Some functional limitations on the course of cognitive growth. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 544–573.Google Scholar
  14. Chelune, G.J., & Baer, R.A. (1986). Developmental norms for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neurospychology, 8, 219–228.Google Scholar
  15. Comalli, P.E., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop Color-Word test in children, adulthood and aging. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47–53.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Cragg, B.C. (1975). The density of synapses and neurons in normal, mentally defective and aging human brains. Brain, 98, 81–90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Crowder, R.G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Crowder, R.G. (1989). Modularity and dissociations in memory systems. In H.L. Roediger, III. & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 271–294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Davies, D.R., Jones, D.M., & Taylor, A. (1984). Selective and sustained-attention tasks: Individual and group differences. In R. Parasuraman, D.R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 395–447). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Dempster, F.N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100.Google Scholar
  21. Dempster, F.N. (1985a). Proactive interference in sentence recall: Topic similarity effects and individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 13, 81–89.Google Scholar
  22. Dempster, F.N. (1985b). Short-term memory development in childhood and adolescence. In C.J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development (pp. 208–248). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  23. Dempster, F.N. (1989). Reflections on the nature and sources of individual differences in learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 1, 1–6.Google Scholar
  24. Dempster, F.N. (in press). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review. Google Scholar
  25. Dempster, F.N. (1991). Inhibitory processes: A neglected dimension of intelligence. Intelligence, 15, 157–173.Google Scholar
  26. Dempster F.N., & Cooney, J.B. (1982). Individual differences in digit span, susceptibility to proactive interference, and aptitude/achievement test scores. Intelligence, 6, 399–416.Google Scholar
  27. Diamond, A. (1988a). Abilities and neural mechanisms underlying AB̄ performance. Child Development, 59, 523–527.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Diamond, A. (1988b). Differences between adult and infant cognition: Is the crucial variable presence or absence of language. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 337–370). Oxford, England: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  29. Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1986). Comparative development of human infants and infant rhesus monkeys of cognitive functions that depend on prefronal cortex. Neurosciences Abstracts, 12, 274.Google Scholar
  30. Doyle, A.B. (1973). Listening to distraction: A development study of selective attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 100–115.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Enns, J.T., & Akhtar, N. (1989). A developmental study of filtering in visual attention. Child Development, 60, 1188–1199.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Enns, J.T., & Cameron, S. (1987). Selective attention in young children: The relations between visual search, filtering, and priming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 38–63.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Fischer, K.W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.Google Scholar
  34. Freedman, M., & Oscar-Berman, M. (1986). Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response deficits in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 337–342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Fuster, J.M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex (2nd ed.). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  36. Gelman, R. (1969). Conservation acquisition: A problem of learning to attend to relevant attributes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 7, 167–187.Google Scholar
  37. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1987). Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. Child Development, 58, 601–622.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Gratch, G. (1976). On levels of awareness of objects in infants and students thereof. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 22, 157–176.Google Scholar
  39. Halford, G.S. (1982). The development of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Harris, P.L. (1973). Perseverative errors in search by young infants. Child Development, 44, 28–33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–224). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  42. Heaton, R.K. (1981). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  43. Hebb, D.O. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology, 21, 73–87.Google Scholar
  44. Hecaen, H., & Albert, M.L. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  45. Howe, M.L., & Brainerd, C.J. (1989). Development of children’s long-term retention. Developmental Review, 9, 301–340.Google Scholar
  46. Howe, M.L., & Rabinowitz, F.M. (1991). Gist another panacea? Or just the illusion of inclusion. Developmental Review, 11, 305–316.Google Scholar
  47. Hudspeth, W.J., & Pribram, K.H. (1990). Stages of brain and cognitive maturation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 881–884.Google Scholar
  48. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex— developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195–205.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Hynd, G.W., & Willis, G. (1985). Neurological foundations of intelligence. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence: Theories, measurements, and applications (pp. 119–157). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  50. Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., Kritchevsky, M., & Squire, L.R. (1989). Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 548–560.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Jensen, A.R., & Rohwer, W.D., Jr. (1966). The Stroop Color-Word test: A review. Acta Psychologica, 25, 36–93.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Kail, R.V., Jr., & Le vine, L.A. (1976). Encoding processes and sex-role preferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 256–263.Google Scholar
  53. LaMantia, A., & Rakic, P. (1984). The number, size, myelination and regional variations in the corpus callosum and anterior commissure of the developing rhesus monkey. Society of Neuroscience Abstracts, 10, 1373.Google Scholar
  54. Lane, D.M., & Pearson, D.A. (1982). The development of selective attention. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 317–337.Google Scholar
  55. Leslie, L. (1979, April). Mediation or production deficiency in disabled readers? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
  56. Lezak, M. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Lindsay, P.H., & Norman, D.A. (1972). Human information processing. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  58. Linn, M.C. (1978). Influence of cognitive style and training on tasks requiring the separation of variables schema. Child Development, 49, 874–877.Google Scholar
  59. Loess, H., & Waugh, N.C. (1967). Short-term memory and inter-trial interval. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 455–460.Google Scholar
  60. Longmore, B.E., & Knight, R.G. (1988). The effect of intellectual deterioration on retention deficits in amnesic alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 448–454.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  62. Luria, A.R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction for neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  63. McGeoch, J. A. (1943). The psychology of human learning. New York: Longmans Green.Google Scholar
  64. Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 313–334). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  65. Moely, B.E. (1977). Organizational factors in the development of memory. In R.V. Kail, Jr., & J.W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 203–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  66. Murdock, B.B. (1974). Human memory: Theory and data. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
  67. Oscar-Berman, M. (1980). Neuropsychological consequences of long-term chronic alcoholism. American Scientist, 68, 410–419.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Parkin, A.J., & Leng, N.R.C. (1987). Aetiological variation in the amnesic syndrome. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 2, pp. 16–21). Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
  69. Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologia, 63, 301–345.Google Scholar
  70. Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). An organismic process model of Witkins’s field-dependence-independence. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36–70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  71. Pascual-Leone, J., Ammon, P., Goodman, D., & Subleman, I. (1978). Piagetian theory and Neo-Piagetion analysis as psychological guides in education. In J.M. Gallagher & J. Easley (Eds.), Knowledge and development: Piaget and education (Vol. 2). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  72. Pavlov, I.P. (1949). Complete Collected Works (Vols. 1–6). Moscow: Nauk SSSR.Google Scholar
  73. Peterson, L.R., & Peterson, M.J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323–330.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  76. Plude, D.J., & Hoyer, W.J. (1985). Attention and performance: Identifying and localizing age deficits. In N. Charness (Ed.), Aging and human performance (pp. 47–99). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  77. Rand, G., Wapner, S., Werner, H., & McFarland, J.H. (1963). Age differences in performance on the Stroop Color-Word test. Journal of Personality, 31, 534–558.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Reinis, S., & Goldman, J.M. (1980). The development of the brain. Sprinfield, II: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
  79. Reyna, V.F., & Brainerd, C.J. (1991). Fuzzy-trace theory and children’s acquisition of mathematical and scientific concepts. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 27–59.Google Scholar
  80. Rourke, B.P., Bakker, D.J., Fisk, J.L., & Stang, J.D. (1983). Child neuropsychology: An introduction to theory, research and clinical practice. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  81. Sarnat, H.B., & Netsky, M.G. (1981). Evolution of the nervous system (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Schacter, L.L., & Moscovitch, M. (1984). Infants, amnesiacs, and dissociable memory systems. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Infant memory (pp. 173–216). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  83. Schacter, D.L., Moscovitch, M., Tulving, E., McLachlan, D.R., & Frendman, M. (1986). Mnemonic precedence in amnesiac patients: An analogue of the AB̄ error in infants. Child Development, 57, 816–823.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Sejnowski, T.J., & Rosenberg, C.R. (1988). Learning and representation in connectionist models. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Perspectives in memory research (pp. 135–178). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  85. Siegler, R.S. (1984). Mechanisms of cognitive growth: Variation and selection. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 141–162). New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  86. Siegler, R.S. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. In M.R. Rosenzweig & L.W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 353–379). Palo Alto, CA. Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
  87. Sophian, C., Larking, J.H., & Kadane, J.B. (1985). A developmental model of search: Stochastic estimation of children’s rule use. In H.M. Wellman (Ed.), Children’s searching (pp. 185–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  88. Sophian, C., & Wellman, H.M. (1983). Selective information use and perseveration in the search behavior of infants and young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 369–390.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Stamm, J.S. (1987). The riddle of the monkey’s delayed response deficit has been solved. In E. Perceman (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 73–89). New York: The IRBN Press.Google Scholar
  90. Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.Google Scholar
  91. Strutt, G.F., Anderson, D.R., & Well, A.D. (1975). A developmental study of the effects of irrelevant information on speeded classification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,20, 127–135.Google Scholar
  92. Stuss, D.T., & Benson, D.F. (1984). Neuropsychological Studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 3–28.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Stuss, D.T., Benson, D.F., Kaplan, E.F., Weir, W.S., Naeser, M.A., Lieberman, I., & Ferrill, D. (1983). The involvement of orbitofrontal cerebrum in cognitive tasks. Neuropsychologia, 21, 235–248.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Stuss, D.T., Kaplan, E.F., Benson, D.F., Weir, W.S., Chivilli, S., & Sarazin, F.F. (1982). Evidence for the involvement of orbitofrontal cortex in memory fonctions: An interference effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 96, 913–925.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Teuber, H.L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 410–444). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  96. Teuber, H.L. (1972). Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiologica Experimenta, 32, 615–656.Google Scholar
  97. Tipper, S.P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.Google Scholar
  98. Tipper, S.P., Bourque, T.A., Anderson, S.H., & Brehaut, J.C. (1989). Mechanisms of attention: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 353–378.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Waber, D. (1989). The biological boundaries of cognitive styles: A neuropsychological analysis. In T. Golberson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 11–35). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  100. Wallach, L., Wall, A.J., & Anderson, L. (1967). Number conservation: The roles of reversibility, addition-subtraction, and misleading perceptual cues. Child Development, 38, 425–442.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
  102. Weinberger, D.R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 660–669.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Winer, G.A., Hemphill, J., & Craig, R.K. (1988). The effect of misleading questions in promoting nonconservation responses in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 24, 197–202.Google Scholar
  104. Wise, L.A., Sutton, J.A., & Gibbons, P.O. (1975). Decrement in Stroop interference time with age. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 149–150.Google Scholar
  105. Witkin, H.A., Dyk, R.B., Faterson, G.E., Goodenough, D.R., & Karp, S.A. (1962). Psychological differentiation. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
  106. Yakovlev, P.I., & LeCours, A.R. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 3–70). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  107. Zelniker, T. (1989). Cognitive style and dimensions of information processing. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 172–191). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  108. Zimilies, H. (1963). A note on Piaget’s concept of conservation. Child Development, 34, 691–695.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Frank N. Dempster

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations