Symbolic Processing of Youth and Elders

  • Kathryn L. Jepson
  • Gisela Labouvie-Vief

Abstract

Whereas cognitive aging research often suggests that extensive and irreversible deficits in the information processing system occur as a natural part of aging (Kausler, 1982; Poon, 1985; Salthouse, 1985), several authors recently have asked if, in addition to decremental patterns, aging also may bring adaptive changes. According to that view (for discussion, see Labouvie-Vief & Schell, 1982), some apparent deficits may be the result of an assumption inherent in many information processing approaches—namely, that no major cognitive qualitative changes occur after early adulthood. As an inevitable consequence, evidence that mature and older adults process information qualitatively differently is interpreted as but another instance of a long list of later-life deficits.

Keywords

Lost Clarification Metaphor Veri Prose 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, C. (1986). Qualitative changes in text memory from adolescence to adulthood. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI.Google Scholar
  2. Adams, C., Labouvie-Vief, G., Hobart, C., & Dorosz, M. (1990). Adult age group differences in story recall style. Journal of Gerontology, 45, pp. 17–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Arlin, P. (1989). Problem solving and problem finding in young artists and young scientists. In M.L Commons, J.D. Sinnott, F.A Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development: Vol. 1. Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 197–216 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  4. Birren, J.E. (1979). Age and decision strategies. In A.T. Welford & J.E. Birren (Eds.), Decision making and age: Interdisciplinary topics in gerontology (Vol. 4, pp. 23–36 ). Basel Switzerland: S. Karger.Google Scholar
  5. Blanchard-Fields, F. (1986). Reasoning on social dilemmas varying in emotional saliency: An adult developmental perspective. Psychology and Aging, 1, 325–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boswell, D.A. (1979). Metaphoric processing in the mature years. Human Development, 22, 373–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown, A.L., & Smiley, S.S. (1977). Rating the importance of structural units of prose passages: A problem of metacognitive development. Child Development, 48, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chinen, A.B. (1985). Fairy tales and transpersonal development in later life. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  9. Chinen, A.B. (1987). Fairy tales and psychological development in late life: A cross-cultural hermeneutic study. The Gerontologist, 27, 340–346.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chinen, A.B. (1989). In the ever after: Fairy tales and the second half of life. Willmette, IL: Chiron Publications.Google Scholar
  11. Colby, A., Kohlberg, L., Gibbs, J., & Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral development. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 49, (2, Serial No. 206).Google Scholar
  12. Cole, J. (1982). Best-loved folktales of the world, New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  13. Commons, M.L., Richards, F.A., & Armon, C. (1984). Beyond formal operations. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  14. Commons, M.L., Richards, F.A., & Kuhn, D. (1982). Systematic, metasystematic, and cross-paradigmatic reasoning: A case for stages of reasoning beyond Piaget’s stage of formal operations. Child Development, 53, 1058–1068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Denney, N.W. (1982). Aging and cognitive changes. In B.B. Wohlman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 807–827 ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  16. DeWit, G.A. (1963). Symbolism of masculinity and femininity: An empirical phenomenological approach to developmental aspects of symbolic thought in word associations and symbolic meanings of words. New York: Springer Publishing.Google Scholar
  17. Fry, E. (1968). A readability formula that saves time. Journal of Reading, 11, 513–516, 575–578.Google Scholar
  18. Hultsch, D.F., & Dixon, R.A. (1984). Memory for text materials in adulthood. In P.B. Baltes & O.G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 77–108 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Jastak, J., & Jastak, S. (1964). Short forms of the WAIS and WISC vocabulary subtests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 167–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Johnson, R.E. (1970). Recall of prose as a function of the structural importance of the linguistic unit. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 12–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kausler, D.H. (1982). Experimental psychology and human aging. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  22. Kitchener, K.S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition, and epistemic cognition. Human Development, 26, 222–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kitchener, K.S., & King, P.M. (1981). Reflective judgment: Concepts of justification and their relation to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kitchener, K.S., King, P.M., Wood, P.K, & Davison, M.L. (1989). Consistency and sequentiality in the development of reflective judgment: A six year longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 73–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kogan, N. (1974). Categorizing and conceptualizing styles in young and older adults. Human Development, 17, 218–230.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kramer, D.A. (1983). Post-formal operations? A need for further conceptualization. Human Development, 26, 91–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kramer, D.A., & Woodruff, D.S. (1984). Breadth of categorization and metaphoric processing: A study of young and older adults. Research on Aging, 6, 271–286.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kuhn, D., Pennington, N., & Leadbeater, B. (1983). Adult thinking in developmental perspective. In P.B. Baltes & O.G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 158–195 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  29. Labouvie-Vief, G., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1982). Cognitive aging and psychological growth. Ageing and Society, 2, 183–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Labouvie-Vief, G., Campbell, S.O., Weaverdyck, S.E., & Tanenhaus, M.K. (1980). Metaphoric processing in young and old adults. Unpublished manuscript, Wayne State University.Google Scholar
  31. Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425–437.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Labouvie-Vief, G., & Hakim-Larson, J. (1989). Developmental shifts in adult thought. In S. Hunter & M. Sundel (Eds.), Midlife myths: Issues, findings, and practical implications (pp. 69–96 ). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  33. Labouvie-Vief, G., & Schell D.A. (1982). Learning and memory in later life. In B.B. Wohlman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 828–846 ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentic-Hall.Google Scholar
  34. Labouvie-Vief, G., Schell, D.A., & Weaverdyck, S.E. (1981). Recall deficit in the aged: A fable recalled. Unpublished manuscript, Wayne State University.Google Scholar
  35. Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  36. Neugarten, B.L. (1968). The awareness of middle age. In B.L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging (pp. 93–98 ). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  37. Perry, W.G. (1968). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
  38. Poon, L.W. (1985). Differences in human memory with aging: Nature, causes and clinical implications. In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging ( 2nd ed., pp. 427–462 ). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  39. Riegel, K.F. (1973). Dialectical operations: The final period of cognitive development. Human Development, 76, 346–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Salthouse, T. (1985). A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
  41. Schaie, K.W. (1990). Intellectual development in adulthood. In J. E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging ( 3rd ed., pp. 291–309 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  42. Shrout, P.E., & Fleiss, J.L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sinnott, J.D. (1989). Life-span relativistic postformal thought: Methodology and data from everyday problem solving studies. In M.L. Commons, J.D. Sinnott, F.A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development: Vol. 1. Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 239–278 ). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kathryn L. Jepson
  • Gisela Labouvie-Vief

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations