Skip to main content

Visual Information Processing and the Menstrual Cycle

  • Chapter
Cognition and the Menstrual Cycle

Part of the book series: Contributions to Psychology and Medicine ((CONTRIBUTIONS))

Abstract

Biological rhythms are time-dependent biological changes that recur at a particular frequency in terms of their duration and in terms of the number of oscillations they comprise. The menstrual cycle is a low-frequency biological rhythm, having a relatively long duration of about 28–30 days, compared with, for instance, medial-frequency circadian rhythms, which have a duration of about 24 hours, or high-frequency rhythms, with a duration of less than half an hour, such as those observed in electroencephalogram and electrocardiogram measurements (Smolensky, 1980).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abplanalp, J. M., Livingston, L., Rose, R. M., & Sandwisch, D. (1977). Cortisol and growth hormone responses to psychological stress during the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 39, 158–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ainscough, A. E. (1990). Premenstrual emotional changes in the older woman. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 12–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altenhaus, A. L. (1978). The effect of expectancy for change on performance during the menstrual cycle. Dissertation Abstracts International, 39, 968B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armour, S., & Asso, D. (1986). Use of two self-report measures of psychophysiological activity in two phases of the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 22, 137–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D. (1978). Levels of arousal in the premenstrual phase. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 47–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D. (1983). The real menstrual cycle. Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D. (1986). The relationship between menstrual cycle changes in nervous system activity and psychological, behavioural and physiological variables. Biological Psychology, 1986, 23, 52–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D. (1987). Cyclical variations. In M. A. Baker (Ed.), Sex differences in human performance (pp. 55–80). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D., & Beech, H. R. (1975). Susceptibility to the acquisition of a conditioned response in relation to the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 19, 337–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D., & Braier J. R. (1982). Changes with the menstrual cycle in psychophysiological and self-report measures of activation. Biological Psychology, 15, 95–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baldessarini, R. J. (1975). The basis for amine hypotheses of affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 1087–1089.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bancroft, J., Sanders, D., Davidson, D., & Warner, P. (1983). Mood, sexuality, hormones and the menstrual cycle: III. Sexuality and the role of androgens. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 509–516.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barris, M. C., Dawson, W. W., & Theiss, C. L. (1980). The visual sensitivity of women during the mentrual cycle. Documenta Ophthalmologia, 42, 293–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, D., Creutzfeldt, O. D., Schwibbe, M., & Wuttke, W. (1982). Changes in physiological, EEG and psychological parameters in women during the spontaneous menstrual cycle and following oral contraceptives. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 7, 75–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, B. (1976). Self-reported activation during the premenstrual and menstrual phases of the menstrual cycle. Journal of Interdisciplinary Cycle Research, 7, 193–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belmaker, R. H., Murphy, D. L., Wyatt, R. J., & Loriaux, L. (1974). Human platelet monoamine oxidase changes during the menstrual cycle. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 553–556.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bobon, D. P., Lecoq, A., von Renckell, R., & Lavergne, G. (1982). Critical flicker fusion frequency: Introduction to the CINP session in Goteborg and methodological recommendations. Pharmacopsychiatria, 15 (Suppl. 1), 1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braier, J. R., & Asso, D. (1980). Two flash fusion as a measure of changes in cortical activation with the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 11, 153–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broverman, D. M., Vogel, W., Klaiber, E. L., Majcher, D., Shea, D., & Paul, V. (1981). Changes in cognitive task performance across the menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 646–654.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buhler, R. A. (1954). Flicker fusion threshold and anxiety level. Dissertation Abstracts, 14, 1255B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clare, G., Tong, J. E., Lyon, R. G., & Leigh, G. (1976). Menstrual cycle and ethanol effects on temporal discrimination. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42, 1085–1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clyde, C. A. (1982). The relationship between personality and the effect of benzodiazepine derivatives on psychomotor performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Leeds, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A., Eneroth, P., & Landgren, B. M. (1985). Psychoneuroendocrine stress response and mood as related to the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 512–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A., Lundstrom, V., Nordstrom, L., Nilsson, C. G., Hamfelt, A., & Eneroth, P. (1986). Psychoneuroendocrinological characteristics of premenstrual tension and treatment with vitamin B6. In L. Dennerstein & I. Frazer (Eds.), Hormones and behaviour (pp. 183–196). Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colquhoun, W. P. (1971). Circadian variations in mental efficiency. In W. P. Colquhoun (Ed.), Biological rhythms and human performance (pp. 39–107). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, J. R. (1980). Hormonal influence on auditory function. Ear and Hearing, 1, 219–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Creutzfeldt, O. D., Arnold, P.-M., Becker, D, Langenstein, S., Tirsch, W., Wilhelm, H., & Wuttke, W. (1976). EEG changes during spontaneous and controlled menstrual cycles and their correlation with psychological performance. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 40, 113–131.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, S. (1990). Critical flicker fusion techniques in psychopharmacology. In I. Hindmarch & P. Stonier (Eds.), Human psychopharmacology: Measures and methods (Vol. 3). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dan, A. J. (1979). The menstrual cycle and sex-related differences in cognitive variability. In A. Petersen & M. Wittig (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning (pp. 241–260). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Marchi, W. G. (1976). Psychophysical aspects of the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 20, 279–289.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Marchi, W. G., & Tong, J. E. (1972). Menstrual, diurnal and activation effects on resolution of temporally paired flashes. Psychophysiology, 9, 362–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennerstein, L., Spencer-Gardner, C., & Burrows, G. D. (1984). Mood and the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 18, 1–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, M., Diamond, A. L., & Mast, M. (1972). Visual sensitivity and sexual arousal levels during the menstrual cycle. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 55, 170–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doty, R. L., Snyder, P. J., Huggins, G. R., & Lowry, D. D. (1981). Endocrine, cardiovascular, and psychological correlates of olfactory sensitivity changes during the human menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 45–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dye, L. (1989). Variations in CFFT during the menstrual cycle: The effects of benzodiazepines. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Leeds, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engel, P., & Hildebrandt, G. (1974). Rhythmic variations in reaction time, heart rate, and blood pressure at different durations of the menstrual cycle. In M. Ferin, F. Halberg, R. M. Richart, & R. L. VandeWiele (Eds.), Biorhythms and human reproduction (pp. 325–334). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, E., Bossola, P. A., Rea, A., Braga, A. R., Tamborini, M., & Romano, S. (1979). Circadian periodicity and ACTH: Response of the adrenal cortex in different phases of the menstrual cycle in normal women. In L. Carenza & L. Zichella (Eds.), Emotion and reproduction (Vol. 20A, pp. 205–213). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frewer, L. J. (1986). Some psychopharmacological variables affecting the critical flicker fusion threshold. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Leeds, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frewer, L. J., & Hindmarch, I. (1988). The effects of time of day, age and anxiety on a choice reaction time task. In I. Hindmarch, B. Aufdembrinke, & H. Ott (Eds.), Psychopharmacology and reaction time (pp. 103–114). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. C., Hurt, S. W., Arnott, M. S., & Clarkin, J. E. (1980). Behavior and the menstrual cycle. Signs, 5, 719–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, J., & Meares, R. A. (1978). Comparison of spontaneous and contraceptive menstrual cycles on a visual discrimination task. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 233–239.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gandelman, R. (1983). Gonadal hormones and sensory function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 7, 1–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gannon, L. (1981). Evidence for a psychological etiology of menstrual disorders: A critical review. Psychological Reports, 48, 287–294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, K. F., & Elder, S. T. (1984). The menstrual cycle from a bio-behavioral approach: A comparison of oral contraceptive and contraceptive users. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1, 209–214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, S. (1955). Flicker fusion measurements and anxiety level. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 200–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golub, S. (1976). The magnitude of premenstrual anxiety and depression. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 4–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golub, S. (1980). Premenstrual changes in mood, personality and cognitive function. In A. J. Dan, E. A. Graham, & C. P. Beecher (Eds.), The menstrual cycle: Vol. 1. A synthesis of interdisciplinary research (pp. 237–246). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gortelmeyer, R., & Wiemann, H. (1982). Retest reliability and construct validity of critical flicker fusion frequency. Pharmacopsychiatrica, 15 (Suppl. 1), 24–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, E. A. (1980). Cognition as related to menstrual cycle phase and estrogen level. In A. J. Dan, E. A. Graham, & C. P. Beecher (Eds.), The menstrual cycle: Vol. 1. A synthesis of interdisciplinary research (pp. 190–208). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, M., & Gaston, J. B. (1978). Changes in auditory perception in the menstrual cycle. British Journal of Audiology, 12, 105–118.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, F. J. (1951). The relationship of odour, taste, and flicker fusion thresholds to food intake. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 403–411.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, W. G., & Russell, J. W. (1986). A prospective comparison study of premenstrual symptoms. Medical Journal of Australia, 144, 466–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, L. R., Lyons, D., & Dunne, M. (1987). Memory and menstrual cycle. Ergonomics, 30, 111–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, L. R., & Shirley, E. (1976). Color-name interference at different times of the day. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 119–122.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, D. (1987). Rhythm and blues: Neurochemical, neuropharmacological, and neuropsychological implications of a hypothesis of circadian rhythm dysfunction in the affective disorders. Psychopharmacology, 93, 271–285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, D., & Williams, J. M. G. (1988). Dysrhythmia, dysphoria, and depression: The interaction of learned helplessness and circadian dysrhythmia in the pathogenesis of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 163–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henkin, R. (1974). Sensory changes during the menstrual cycle. In M. Ferin, F. Halberg, R. M. Richart, & R. L. VandeWiele (Eds.), Biorhythms and human reproduction (pp. 227–286). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. J., Walsh, R. D., & Hindmarch, I. (1981). Tolerability of nocturnal doses of Clobazam in anxious patients in general practice. Royal Society of Medicine International Congress and Symposium Series, 43, 133–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch, I. (1979). A preliminary study of the effects of repeated doses of clobazam on aspects of performance, arousal and behaviour in a group of anxiety rated volunteers. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 16, 17–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch, I. (1980). Psychomotor function and psychoactive drugs. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 10, 189–209.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch, I. (1982). Critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF): The effect of psychotropic compounds. Pharmacopsychiatria, 15 (Suppl. 1), 44–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch, I. (1984). Measuring the effects of psychoactive drugs. In S. Campbell-Smith (Ed.), Readings in psychiatry 3. Oxford, England: Medical Education Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch, I. (1988). Information processing, psychological performance and anxiolytic drugs. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 64 (Suppl. 2), 10–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, H.-Z., Gilger, J. W., & Brink, T. M. (1986). Effects of menstrual cycle on spatial information-processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 743–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, S., Schraer, R., Landers, D. M., Buskirk, E. R., & Harris, D. V. (1979). The effects of total oestrogen concentration and menstrual cycle phase on reaction time performance. Ergonomics, 22, 213–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenner, F. A. (1988). More than a hundred years of studies of periodicity in psychiatry. In V. Marks & S. Parker (Eds.), The biological clock: Current approaches (pp. 1–8). Southampton, England: Duphar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, B. K. (1982). Menstrual cycle effects on task performance. Acta Psychologica, 50, 159–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, O. (1958). Relationship between visual and auditory discrimination and anxiety level. Journal of General Psychology, 59, 111–118.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klaiber, E. L., Broverman, D. M., Vogel, N., & Kobayashi, Y. (1974). Rhythms in plasma MAO activity, EEG, and behavior during the menstrual cycle. In M. Ferin, F. Halberg, R. M. Richart, & R. L. VandeWiele (Eds.), Biorhythms and human reproduction (pp. 353–370). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and wakefulness (rev. ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koeske, R. K. D., & Koeske, G. F. (1975). An attributional approach to moods and the menstrual cycle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 473–478.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kopell, B. S., Lunde, D. T., Clayton, R. B., & Moos R. H. (1969). Variations in some measures of arousal during the menstrual cycle. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 148, 180–187.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krugman, H. (1947). Flicker fusion frequency as a function of anxiety reaction: An exploratory study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 4, 269–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczmierczyk, A. R., & Adams, H. E. (1986). Autonomic arousal and pain sensitivity in women with premenstrual syndrome at different phases of the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 30, 421–428.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahmeyer, H. W., Miller, M., & DeLeon-Jones, F. (1982). Anxiety and mood fluctuation during the normal menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 183–194.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landis, C., & Hamwi, V. (1954). The effect of certain physiological determinants on the flicker fusion threshold. Journal of Applied Physiology, 6, 566–571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levander, S. E., & Lageren, K. (1973). Four vigilance indicators to use with a minicomputer (Rep. No. 381). Stockholm: University of Stockholm, Psychological Laboratories.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, B. C., & Zahn, T. P. (1974). Changes in mood and autonomic functioning during the menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 11, 579–590.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loucks, J., & Thompson, H. (1968). Effect of menstruation on reaction time. Research Quarterly, 39, 407–408.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett-Doust, J. W., & Podineks, I. (1975). Comparison between some biological clocks regulating aspects of sensory and motor perception in man. Neuropsychobiology, 1, 261–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackinnon, P. C. B., & Harrison, J. (1961). The influence of hormones associated with pituitary-adrenal and sexual-cycle activity on palmar sweating. Journal of Endocrinology, 23, 217–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marinari, K. T., Leshner, A. I., & Doyle, M. P. (1976). Menstrual cycle status and adrenocortical reactivity to psychological stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1, 213–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClintock, M. K. (1971). Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature, 229, 244–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Millodot, M., & Lamont, A. (1974). Influence of menstruation on corneal sensitivity. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 58, 752–756.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moos, R. H., & Leiderman, D. (1978). Toward a menstrual cycle symptomatology. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 22, 31–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morin, J., Fitzgerald, S., & Zucker, I. (1977). Estrogen shortens circadian rhythm in rats. Science, 196, 305–307.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Musemeci, M., & Misiak, H. (1974). Circadian variation of CFF among schoolchildren. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 751–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K. (1974). Circadian rhythms and representations of the environment in motivational systems. In D. J. McFarland (Ed.), Motivational control systems analysis (pp. 427–459). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Goodwin, B. C. (1971). The explanation and investigation of biological rhythms. In W. P. Colquhoun (Ed.), Biological rhythms and human performance (pp. 1–38). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1974). Stereotypic beliefs about menstruation: A methodological note on the Moos menstrual distress questionnaire and some new data. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 229–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1980). Positive changes in moods and activation levels during the menstrual cycle in experimentally naive subjects. In A. J. Dan, E. A. Graham, & C. P. Beecher (Eds.), The menstrual (ycle: Vol. 1. A synthesis of interdisciplinary research (pp. 247–263). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1982). The psychology of the menstrual cycle: Biological and physiological perspectives. In R. C. Friedman (Ed.), Behavior and the menstrual cycle (pp. 77–99). New York: Marcel Dekker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (1983). Menstrual rhythms in sensory processes: A review of fluctuations in vision, olfaction, audition, taste and touch. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 539–548.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, A. C. (1982). Critical Flicker Fusion Thresholds and their relationship to other measures of alertness. Pharmacopsychiatrica, 15 (Suppl. 1), 39–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patkai, P., Johannsson, G., & Post, B. (1974). Mood, alertness and sympatheticadrenal medullary activity during the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 503–512.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierson, W. R., & Lockhart, A. (1963). Effect of menstruation on simple reaction and movement time. British Medical Journal, 1, 796–797.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ponciano, E., Relvas, J., Mendes, F., Lameiras, A., Vaz Serra, A., & Hindmarch, I. (1981). Clinical effects and sedative activity of Bromazepam and Clobazam in the treatment of anxious out-patients. Royal Society of Medicine International Congress and Symposium Series, 43, 125–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, F. S., Bateman, S. C., Short, R. V., & Wilkinson, R. T. (1974). The effects of flying and of time changes on menstrual cycle length and on performance in airline stewardesses. In M. Ferin, F. Halberg, R. M. Richart, & R. L. Vande-Wiele (Eds.), Biorhythms and human reproduction (pp. 501–512). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quadagno, D. M., Shubeita, H. E., Deck, J., & Francoeur, D. (1981). Influence of male social contacts, exercise, and all female living conditions on the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 6, 239–244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reinberg, A., & Smolensky, M. H. (1974). Circatrigintan secondary rhythms related to hormonal changes in the menstrual cycle: General considerations. In M. Ferin, F. Halberg, R. M. Richart, & R. L. VandeWiele (Eds.), Biorhythms and human reproduction (pp. 241–259). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, C. (1965). Biological clocks in medicine and psychiatry. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodin, J. (1976). Menstruation, reattribution, and competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 345–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. E. (1977). Body time and social time: Mood patterns by menstrual cycle phase and day of the week. Social Science Research, 6, 273–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Clarke, A. (1980). Research on menstrual-related psychological changes: Alternative perspectives. In J. E. Parsons (Ed.), The psychobiology of sex differences and sex roles (pp. 227–243). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, D., Warner, P., Backstrom, T., & Bancroft J. (1983). Mood, sexuality and the menstrual cycle: I. Changes in mood and physical state: Description of subjects and method. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 487–501.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scher, D., Pionk, M., & Purcell, D. G. (1981). Visual sensitivity fluctuations during the menstrual cycle under dark and light adaptation. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 159–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scher, D., Purcell, D. G., & Caputo, S. J. (1985). Visual acuity at two phases of the menstrual cycle. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 119–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, G. W., Meyer, R. C., & Washer, S. H. (1975). Responses to short-duration signals, pre and post menses, in subjects using oral contraceptive and subjects not using oral contraceptive. Journal of the American Audiology Society, 1, 112–118.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shader, R. L, DiMascio, A., & Harmatz, J. (1968). Characterological anxiety levels and premenstrual libido changes. Psychosomatics, 9, 197–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, P. V., Gerathwohl, S. J., & Mohler, S. R. (1969). Time zone effects: Disruption of circadian rhythms poses a stress on the long distance air traveller. Science, 164, 1246–1255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonsen, E., & Brozek, J. (1952). Flicker fusion frequency: Background and applications. Physiological Review, 32, 349–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, P., & Jenner, F. A. (1979). Autonomic activity in subjects reporting changes in affect in the menstrual cycle. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 135–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. M., & Misiak, H. (1976). Critical flicker frequency (CFF) and psychotropic drugs in normal human subjects: A review. Psychopharmacology, 47, 175–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, M. H. (1980). Chronobiologic considerations in the investigation and interpretation of circamensual rhythms in women. In A. J. Dan, E. A. Graham, & C. P. Beecher (Eds.), The menstrual cycle: Vol. 1. A synthesis of interdisciplinary research (pp. 76–90). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, B. (1973). The effect of menstruation on cognitive and perceptual-motor behavior: A review. Psychosomatic Medicine, 35, 515–534.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, B. (1982). Cognitive behavior and the menstrual cycle. In R. C. Friedman (Ed.), Behavior and the menstrual cycle (pp. 101–127). New York: Marcel Dekker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, B. (1983). How does menstruation affect cognitive competence and psychophysiological response? In S. Golub (Ed.), Women and health: 8. Lifting the curse of menstruation (pp. 53–90). New York: Haworth Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenn, P. G., & Klinge, V. (1972). Relationship between the menstrual cycle and bodily activity in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 3, 297–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, B., & Appelt, H. (1983). Psychological concomitants of the menstrual cycle: A prospective longitudinal approach. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 2, 215–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, B., Schultheiss, M., & Cohen, R. (1983). Autonomic reactivity in the premenstrual phase. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 1–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. E. (1967). Measurement of activation through self-report. Psychological Reports, 20, 663–678.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, R. E. (1970). Activation states as assessed by verbal report and 4 physiological variables. Psychophysiology, 7, 86–95.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Udry, J. R., & Morris, N. M. (1968). Distribution of coitus in the menstrual cycle. Nature, 220, 593–596.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uno, T. (1972). GSR activity and the human menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 10, 213–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ussher, J. M. (1989). The psychology of the female body. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Akker, O. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of menstrual cycle distress. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vila, J., & Beech, H. R. (1978). Vulnerability and defensive reactions in relation to the human menstrual cycle. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17, 93–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voda, A. (1980). Pattern of progesterone and aldosterone in ovulating women during the menstrual cycle. In A. J. Dan, E. A. Graham, & C. P. Beecher (Eds.), The menstrual cycle: Vol. 1. A synthesis of interdisciplinary research (pp. 223–236). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, W., Broverman, D. M., & Klaiber, E. L. (1971). EEG responses in regularly menstruating women and amenorrheic women treated with ovarian hormones. Science, 172, 388–391.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagoner, R. A. (1960). Differences in response latency and response variability between high and low anxiety subjects in a flicker fusion task. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61, 355–359.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, J. F., & Misiak, H. (1966). Diurnal variation of critical flicker frequencies. Journal of General Psychology, 75, 167–175.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, M. M. Stone, S. C., & Sandman, C. A. (1978). Visual perception in women during the menstrual cycle. Physiology and Behavior, 20, 239–243.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, J., & Elmadjian, F. (1962). Excretion of epinephrine and norepinephrine in premenstrual tension. Federation Proceedings, 21, 184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcoxon, L. A., Schrader, S. L., & Sherif, C. W. (1976). Daily self-reports on activities, life events, moods, and somatic changes during the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 399–417.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wineman, E. W. (1971). Autonomic balance changes during the human menstrual cycle. Psychophysiology, 8, 1–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winfree, A. J. (1983). Circadian rhythms: Hamsters without jet-lag. Nature, 336, 311–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, S., & Tong, J. E. (1974). Menstrual cycle and contraceptive hormonal effects on temporal discrimination. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 103–108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, N. F., Most, A., & Dery, G. K. (1982). Prevalence of perimenstrual symptoms. American Journal of Public Health, 72, 1257–1264.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wuttke, W., Arnold, P., Becker, D., Creutzfeldt, O., Langenstein, S., & Tirsch, W. (1975). Circulating hormones, EEG, and performance in psychological tests of women, with and without oral contraceptives. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1, 141–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamori, Y., Lorenberg, W., & Sjoerdsma, A. (1970). Norepinephrine metabolism in brainstem of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Science, 170, 544–546.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, E., & Parlee, M. B. (1973). Behavioral changes associated with the menstrual cycle: An experimental investigation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 3, 335–344.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dye, L. (1992). Visual Information Processing and the Menstrual Cycle. In: Richardson, J.T.E. (eds) Cognition and the Menstrual Cycle. Contributions to Psychology and Medicine. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9148-7_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9148-7_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4613-9150-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-9148-7

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics