Sequential Order and Plasticity in Early Psychological Development

  • J. McVicker Hunt
Part of the Topicsin Cognitive Development book series (TOPCOGDEV)

Abstract

The philosophers of nature were exceedingly slow to recognize that organisms go through a series of changes in anatomical substance and structure during their embryonic development. The ancient aphorism that “hair cannot come from not-hair,” epitomizing the principle that no substance or structure can come from a substance or structure of a different nature, dominated human thought from the days of ancient Greece to nearly modern times. Over 2,000 years passed from the time of Aristotle’s first observations of some of the epigenetic changes that occur in chick embryos until Casper Friedrich Wolff (1759, 1768) detailed the transformations in the circulatory system and the intestine of chick embryos so clearly that he convinced at least those informed of biological matters, brought an end to the doctrine of preformationism, and established recognition of the epigenetic nature of embryonic development.

Keywords

Assimilation Hunt Novi 

References

  1. Badger, E. D. A mother’s training program-The road to a purposeful existence. Children, September 1971,18, 168–173. (a).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Badger, E. D. Teaching guide: Infant learning program. Paoli, P.: The Instructo Corporation, A subsidiary of McGraw-Hill, 1971. (b).Google Scholar
  3. Badger, E. D. Teaching guide: Toddler learning program. Paoli, Pa.: The Instructo Corporation, A subsidiary of McGraw-Hill, 1971. (c).Google Scholar
  4. Badger, E. D. A mothers’ training program-A sequel article. Children Today, May 1972,1, 7–11;36.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Berlyne, D. E. What next? Concluding summary. In H. I. Day, D. E. Berlyne, & D. E. Hunt (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education. Toronto: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston of Canada, 1971.Google Scholar
  6. Binet, A., & Simon, T. Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intelleetuel des anormaux. L’année Psychologique, 1905,11, 191–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carmichael, L. The onset and early development of behavior. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1954.Google Scholar
  8. Erikson, E. H. Childhood and Society. New York: Norton, 1950.Google Scholar
  9. Galton, F. Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Macmillan, 1869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gesell, A. The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1954.Google Scholar
  11. Green, B. F. A method of scalogram analysis using summary statistics. Psychometrika, 1956, 21, 79–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Greenberg, D., Uzgiris, I.C., & Hunt, J. McV. Attentional preference and experience: III. Visual familiarity and looking time. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1970, 117, 123–135.Google Scholar
  13. Harlow, H. F. The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 1949,56, 51–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hebb, D. O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949.Google Scholar
  15. Hunt, J. McV. Piaget’s observations as a source of hypotheses concerning motivation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1963,9, 263–275.Google Scholar
  16. Hunt, J. McV. Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 13. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1965.Google Scholar
  17. Hunt, J. McV. The impact and limitations of the giant of developmental psychology. In D. Elkind & J. H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jean Piaget. New York: Oxford University Press, 1969.Google Scholar
  18. Hunt, J. McV. Attentional preference and experience: I. Introduction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1970,117, 99–107.Google Scholar
  19. Hunt, J. McV. Intrinsic motivation and psychological development. In H. M. Schroder & P. Suedfeld (Eds.), Personality theory and information processing. New York: Ronald Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  20. Karnes, M. B., Teska, J. A., Hodgins, A. A., & Badger, E. D. Educational intervention at home by mothers of disadvantaged infants. Child Development, 1970,41, 925–935.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McCarthy, D. Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1954.Google Scholar
  22. Paraskevopoulos, J., & Hunt, J. McV. Object construction and imitation under differing conditions of rearing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1911,119, 301–321.Google Scholar
  23. Piaget, J. [The origins of intelligence in children] (M. Cook, trans.). New York: International Universities Press, 1952. (Originally published, 1936.) (a).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Piaget, J. [Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood] (C. Cattegno & F. M. Hodgson, trans.). New York: Norton, 1952. (Originally published, 1945.) (b).Google Scholar
  25. Piaget, J. [The construction of reality in the child] (M. Cook, trans.). New York: Basic Books, 1954. (Originally published, 1937.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Shirley, M. M. A motor sequence favors the maturation theory. Psychological Bulletin, 1931, 28, 204–205.Google Scholar
  27. Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953.Google Scholar
  28. Stern, W. [Psychological methods of testing intelligence] (G. M. Whipple, trans.). Baltimore: Warwick & York, 1914. (Originally published in 1912.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. McV. Attentional preference and experience: II. An exploratory longitudinal study of the effect of visual familiarity and responsiveness. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1970,117, 109–121.Google Scholar
  30. Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. McV. Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  31. Watson, J. B. Behaviorism. New York: Norton, 1924.Google Scholar
  32. Weizmann, F., Cohen, L. B & Pratt, R. J. Novelty, familiarity, and the development of infant attention. Developmental Psychology, 1911,4, 149–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wetherford, M. J., & Cohen, L. B. Developmental changes in infant visual preferences for novelty and familiarity. Child Development, 1973,44, 416–424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. White, B. L. An experimental approach to the effects of experience on early human development. In J. P. Hill (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1967, pp. 201–226.Google Scholar
  35. Wolff, C. F. Theoria Generations. Halle, 1759.Google Scholar
  36. Wolff, C. F. De formatione intestinorum praecipue, turn et de amnio, aliisque partibus embryonis gallinacei nondum visis. Novi Commentarii Academiae Scientiarum Imperialis Petropolitanae, 1768,12; 1769,13.Google Scholar
  37. Woodworth, R. S. Reinforcement of perception. American Journal of Psychology, 1947,60, 119–124.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1977

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. McVicker Hunt
    • 1
  1. 1.University of IllinoisUrbanaUSA

Personalised recommendations