The Psychological Aesthetics of Narrative Forms

  • Christy Moynihan
  • Albert Mehrabian

Abstract

The study of aesthetic experience may be approached from a number of viewpoints. A work of art is composed of a myriad of elements, any one of which may be examined with respect to its impact on aesthetic preference. A painting may be analyzed in terms of color, form, or subject matter: a story or novel in terms of style, plot, theme, or point of view; a song in terms of melody, rhythm, or harmonal structure, and so forth. The multiplicity of elements in a work of art, as well as the diversity among different art forms, would seem to make it difficult to identify general principles underlying all aesthetic experience. Berlyne (1971, 1974), however, has constructed a general theory of psychological aesthetics which conceptualizes all aesthetic experience, whether in response to visual art, literature, or music, as a unitary phenomenon. This theory is based not on the kinds of elements in a work, but on the degree of interdependence among these elements.

Keywords

Tempo Hines Prose 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abrams, M. H. A glossary of literary terms (3rd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1971.Google Scholar
  2. Arnheim, R. Toward a psychology of art: Collected Essays. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  3. Attneave, F. Application of information theory to psychology: A summary of basic concepts, methods, and results. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1959.Google Scholar
  4. Barron, F. Complexity-simplicity as a personality dimension. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, 48, 163–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barron, F., & Welsh, G. S. Artistic perception as a possible factor in personality style: Its measurement by a figure preference test. Journal of Psychology, 1952, 33, 199–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barthes, R. S/Z: An essay. New York: Hill & Wang, 1974.Google Scholar
  7. Berlyne, D. E. Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berlyne, D. E. Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1971.Google Scholar
  9. Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  10. Berlyne, D. E., & Ogilvie, J. C. Dimensions of perceptions of paintings. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  11. Cantor, G. N., Cantor, J. H., & Ditrichs, R. Observing behavior in preschool children as a function of stimulus complexity. Child Development, 1963, 34, 683–689.Google Scholar
  12. Cherry, C. On human communication: A review, a survey, and a criticism. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  13. Child, I. L. Esthetics. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. III). Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969.Google Scholar
  14. Crozier, J. B. Verbal and exploratory responses to sound sequences varying in uncertainty level. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  15. Culler, J. Structuralist poetics: Structuralism, linguistics, and the study of literature. Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. DE Charms, R. Personal causation. New York: Academic Press, 1968.Google Scholar
  17. Eysenck, H. J. The experimental study of “good Gestalt”: A new approach. Psychological Review, 1942, 49, 344–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. Functions of varied experience. Homewood, Ill,: Dorsey Press, 1961.Google Scholar
  19. Garner, W. R. Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. New York: Wiley, 1962.Google Scholar
  20. Hare, F. G. Verbal responses to visual patterns varying in distributional redundancy and in variety. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  21. Hevner, K. The affective value of pitch and tempo in music. American Journal of Psychology, 1937, 49, 621–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hines, M., & Mehrabian, A. Approach-avoidance behaviors as a function of pleasantness and arousing quality of settings and individual differences in stimulus screening. Social Behavior and Personality, 1979, 7, 223–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kammann, R. Verbal complexity and preferences in poetry. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966, 5, 536–540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kermode, F. The sense of an ending: Studies in the theory of fiction. New York: Oxford University Press, 1967.Google Scholar
  25. Keston, M. J., & Pinto, M. Possible factors influencing musical preference. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1955, 86, 101–113.Google Scholar
  26. Leckart, B. T., & Bakan, P. Complexity judgments of photographs and looking time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 21, 16–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McLuhan, M. Understanding media: The extensions of man. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964.Google Scholar
  28. Mehrabian, A. Public places and private spaces. New York: Basic Books, 1976.Google Scholar
  29. Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. An approach to environmental psychology. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press, 1974. (a)Google Scholar
  30. Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. A verbal measure of information rate for studies in environmental psychology. Environment and Behavior, 1974, 6, 233–252.(b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mehrabian, A., & West, S. Emotional impact of a task and its setting on work performance of screeners and nonscreeners. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1977, 45, 895–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Moynihan, C. Structuring processes and aesthetic preference in the experience of narrative forms. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles, 1980.Google Scholar
  33. Munsinger, H., & Kessen, W. Uncertainty, structure and preference. Psychological Monographs, 1964, 78, (9, Whole No. 586).Google Scholar
  34. Normore, L. F. Verbal responses to visual sequences varying in uncertainty level. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  35. Osborne, J. W., & Farley, F. H. The relationship between aesthetic preference and visual complexity in abstract art. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 49, 69–70.Google Scholar
  36. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  37. Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. Some behavioral effects of the physical environment. In S. Wapner, S. Cohen, & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Experiencing the environment. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  38. Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. Evidence for a three-factor theory of emotions. Journal of Research in Personality, 1977, 11, 273–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. Approach-avoidance and affiliation as functions of the emotion-eliciting quality of an environment. Environment and behavior, 1978, 10, 355–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Scholes, R. Structuralism in literature: An Introduction. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  41. Scott, W. S. (Ed.) Five Approaches of literary criticism: An arrangement of contemporary critical essays. New York: Macmillan, 1962.Google Scholar
  42. Shklovsky, V. La construction de la nouvelle et du roman. In T. Todorov (Ed.), Théorie de la littérature. Paris: Seuil, 1965.Google Scholar
  43. Stolnitz, J. Aesthetics and philosophy of art criticism: A critical introduction. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1960.Google Scholar
  44. Valentine, C. W. The experimental psychology of beauty. New York: Barnes & Noble, 1962.Google Scholar
  45. Vitz, P. C. Affect as a function of stimulus variation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 71, 74–79.(a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Vitz, P. C. Preference for different amounts of visual complexity. Behavioral Science, 1966, 11, 105–114.(b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Walker, E. I. Complexity and preference in animals and men. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1970, 169, 619–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Wexner, L. B. The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood tones. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1954, 38, 432–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wohlwill, J. F. Amount of stimulus exploration and preference as differential functions of stimulus complexity. Perception and Psychophysics, 1968, 4, 307–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1981

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christy Moynihan
    • 1
  • Albert Mehrabian
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations