A Neuropsychological Theory of Anxiety

  • J. A. Gray
Part of the Emotions, Personality, and Psychotherapy book series (CISJ)

Abstract

Gray makes it quite clear that readers should be prepared for a novel conception of anxiety that will strike some as paradoxical. He maintains that the two major components of anxiety are conditioned fear and conditioned or anticipatory frustration. It is his data-supported argument that these two phenomena can be viewed as separate states or as a single state (anxiety) that will create a paradox for some readers. Gray resolves the paradox by demonstrating the functional equivalence of conditioned fear and conditioned frustration in animals.

Keywords

Dopamine Immobilization Noradrenaline Diazepam Stein 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altman, J., Brunner, R. L., & Bayer, S. A. The hippocampus and behavioural maturation. Behavioural Biology, 1973, 8, 557–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amsel, A. Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some recent history and a theoretical extension. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 306–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amsel, A., & Roussel, J. Motivational properties of frustration: I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1952, 43, 363–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anlezark, G. M., Crow, T. J., & Greenway, H. P. Impaired learning and decreased cortical norepinephrine after bilateral locus coeruleus lesions. Science, 1973, 181, 682–684.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defence reactions. In F. R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive conditioning and learning. New York, London: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  6. Bolles, R. C. The avoidance learning problem. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Learning and motivation, Vol. 6. London, New York: Academic Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  7. Broadhurst, P. L. Applications of biomedical genetics to the inheritance of behaviour. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Psychogenetics and psychopharmacology, Vol. 1. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1960.Google Scholar
  8. Broadhurst, P. L. The Maudsley reactive and nonreactive strains of rats: A survey. Behaviour Genetics, 1975, 5, 299–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brown, R. T., & Wagner, A. R. Resistance to punishment and extinction following training with shock or non-reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 68, 503–507.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Capaldi, E. J. A sequential hypothesis of instrumental learning. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 1. New York, London: Academic Press, 1967.Google Scholar
  11. Capaldi, E. J., & Sparling, D. L. Amobarbital and the partial reinforcement effect in rats: Isolating frustrative control over instrumental responding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 74, 467–477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caplan, M. An anlysis of the effects of septal lesions on negatively reinforced behavior. Behavioral Biology, 1973, 9, 129–167.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Caul, W. F. Effects of amobarbital on discrimination acquisition and reversal. Psychopharmacologia, 1967, 77, 414–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Corrodi, H., Fuxe, K., Lidbrink, P., & Olson, L. Minor tranquilizers, stress and central cate-cholamine neurons. Brain Research, 1971, 29, 1–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. DeFrance, J. D. A functional analysis of the septal nuclei. In J. D. DeFrance (Ed.), The septal nuclei. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  16. Douglas, R. J. The hippocampus and behaviour. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 67, 416–442.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Drewett, R. F., Gray, J. A., James, D. T. D., McNaughton, N., Valero, I., & Dudderidge, H. J. Sex and strain differences in septal driving of hippocampal theta rhythm as a function of frequency: Effects of gonadectomy and gonadal hormones. Neuroscience, 1977, 2, 1033–1041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 29, 390–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Feldon, J„ Guillamon, A., Gray, J. A., de Wit, H., & McNaughton, N. Sodium amylobarbitone and responses to nonreward. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
  20. Glazer, H. I. Instrumental conditioning of hippocampal theta and subsequent response persistence. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1974, 86, 267–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Goodrich, K. P. Performance in different segments of an instrumental response chain as a function of reinforcement shedule. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1959, 57, 57–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gray, J. A. Sodium amobarbital and effects of frustrative nonreward. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 55–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gray, J. A. Sodium amobarbital, the hippocampal theta rhythm and the partial reinforcement extinction effect. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 465–480.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gray, J. A. Medial septal lesions, hippocampal theta rhythm and the control of vibrissal movement in the freely moving rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1971, 30, 189–197.(a)Google Scholar
  25. Gray, J. A. The psychology of fear and stress. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson: New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971. (b)Google Scholar
  26. Gray, J. A. The structure of the emotions and the limbic system: a theoretical model. In R. Porter & J. Knight (Eds.), Physiology, emotion and psychosomatic illness. (Ciba Foundation Symposium, 8). Amsterdam: Associated Scientific Publishers, 1972. (a)Google Scholar
  27. Gray, J. A. Effects of septal driving of the hippocampal theta rhythm on resistance to extinction. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 8, 481–490. (b)Google Scholar
  28. Gray, J. A. Elements of a two-process theory of learning. London: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  29. Gray, J. A. The behavioural inhibition system: A possible substrate for anxiety. In M. P. Feldman & A. Broadhurst (Eds.), Theoretical and experimental bases of the behaviour therapies. London: Wiley, 1976.Google Scholar
  30. Gray, J. A. Drug effects on fear and frustration: Possible limbic site of action of minor tranquilizers. In L. Iversen, S. Iversen, & S. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of psychopharmacology, Vol. 8. New York: Plenum Press, 1977, pp. 433–529.Google Scholar
  31. Gray, J. A., Araujo-Silva, M. T., & Quintao, L. Resistance to extiction after partial reinforcement training with blocking of the hippocampal theta rhythm by septal stimulation. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 8, 497–502.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gray, J. A., & Ball, G. G. Frequency-specific relation between hippocampal theta rhythm, behavior, and amobarbital action. Science, 1970, 168, 1246–1248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gray, J. A., McNaughton, N., James, D. T. D., & Kelly, P. H. Effects of minor tranquillizers on hippocampal theta rhythm mimicked by depletion of forebrain noradrenaline. Nature, 1975, 258, 424–425.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Gray, J. A., Quintao, L., & Araujo-Silva, M. T. The partial reinforcement extinction effect in rats with medial septal lesions. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 8, 491–496.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Grossman, S. P. Behavioral function of the septum: A re-analysis. In J. F. DeFrance (Ed.), The septal nuclei. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  36. Haggard, D. F. Acquisition of a simple running response as a function of partial and continuous schedules of reinforcement. Psychological Reports, 1959, 9, 11–18.Google Scholar
  37. Henke, P. G. Persistence of runway performance after septal lesions in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1974, 86, 760–767.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ison, J. R., Glass, D. H., & Bohmer, H. M. Effects of sodium amytal on the approach to stimulus change. Proceedings of the American Psychological Association. 1966, 2, 5–6.Google Scholar
  39. Ison, J. R., & Pennes, E. S. Interaction of amobarbital sodium and reinforcement schedule in determining resistance to extinction of an instrumental running response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 215–219.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Iwahara, S., Iwasaki, T., Nagamura, N., & Masuyama, E. Effect of chlordiazepoxide upon partially-reinforced behavior of rats in the straight runway. Japanese Psychological Research, 1966, 8, 131–135.Google Scholar
  41. Izard, C. E. Patterns of emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression. New York: Academic Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  42. James, D. T. D., McNaughton, N., Rawlins, J. N. P., Feldon, J., & Gray, J. A. Septal driving of hippocampal theta rhythm as a function of frequency in the free-moving male rat. Neuroscience, 1977, 2, 1007–1017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kimsey, R. A., Dyer, R. S., & Petri, H. L. Relationship between hippocampal EEG, novelty and frustration in the rat. Behavioral Biology, 1974, 11, 561–568.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kurtz, R. G. Hippocampal and cortical activity during sexual behavior in the female rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1975, 89, 158–169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Landfield, P. W. Synchronous EEG rhythms: Their nature and their possible functions in memory, information transmission and behavior. In W. H. Gispen (Ed.), Molecular and functional neurobiology. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1976.Google Scholar
  46. Levine, S., Goldman, L., & Coover, G. D. Expectancy and the pituitary-adrenal system. In R. Porter & J. Knight (Eds), Physiology, emotion and psychosomatic illness. Amsterdam: Associated Scientific Publishers, 1972.Google Scholar
  47. Lidbrink, P., Corrodi, H., Fuxe, K., & Olson, L. Barbiturates and meprobamate: Decreases in catecholamine turnover of central dopamine and noradrenaline neuronal systems and the influence of immobilization stress. Brain Research, 1972, 45, 507–524.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lubar, J. F., & Numan, R. Behavioral and physiological studies of septal functions and related medial cortical structures. Behavioral Biology, 1973, 8, 1–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mackintosh, N. J. The psychology of animal learning. London: Academic Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  50. Mason, S. T., & Iversen, S. D Learning in the absence of forebrain noradrenaline. Nature, 1975, 258, 422–424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mason, S. T., & Iversen, S. D. Effects of selective forebrain noradrenaline loss on behavioral inhibition in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1977, 91, 165–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. McCleary, R. A. Response-modulating functions of the limbic system: initiation and suppression. In E. Stellar & J. M. Sprague (Eds.), Progress in physiological psychology, Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  53. McFarland, D. J. The role of attention in the disinhibition of displacement activities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 18, 19–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. McGonigle, B., McFarland, D. J., & Collier, P. Rapid extinction following drug-inhibited incidental learning. Nature, 1967, 214, 531–532.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. McNaughton, N., James, D. T. D., Stewart, J., Gray, J. A., Valero, I., & Drewnowski, A. Septal driving of hippocampal theta rhythm as a function of frequency in the male rat: drug effects. Neuroscience, 1977, 2, 1019–1027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Meibach, R. C., & Siegel, A. Efferent connections of the septal area in the rat: An analysis utilizing retrograde and anterograde transport methods. Brain Research, 1977, 119, 1–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Millenson, J. R. Principles of behavioral analysis. New York: Macmillan, 1967.Google Scholar
  58. Miller, N. E. Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Mowrer, O. H. On the dual nature of learning: A re-interpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving. ” Harvard Educational Review, 1947, 17, 102–148.Google Scholar
  60. Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Myhrer, T. Normal jump avoidance performance in rats with the hippocampal theta rhythm selec-tively disrupted. Behavioral Biology, 1975, 14, 489–498.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Nadel, L., O’Keefe, J., & Black, A. A critique of Altman, Brunner and Bayer’s response-inhibition model of hippocampal function. Behavioral Biology, 1975, 14, 151–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Nelson, P. B., & Wollen, K. A. Effects of ethanol and partial reinforcement upon runway acquisition. Psychonomic Science, 1965, 3, 135–136.Google Scholar
  64. Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes (translated by G. V. Anrep). London: Oxford University Press, 1927.Google Scholar
  65. Plutchik, R. The emotions: Facts, theories and a new model. New York: Random House, 1962.Google Scholar
  66. Raisman, G., Cowan, W. M., & Powell, T. P. S. An experimental analysis of the efferent projections of the hippocampus. Brain, 1966, 89, 83–108.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rolls, E. T. The brain and reward. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  68. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 379–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century, 1938.Google Scholar
  70. Sokolov, Ye. N. Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In M. A. B. Brazier (Ed.), The central nervous system and behaviour, 3rd Conference. New York: Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation, 1960.Google Scholar
  71. Spence, K. W. Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Stein, L. Reciprocal action of reward and punishment mechanisms. In R. G. Heath (Ed.), The role of pleasure in behavior. New York: Harper & Row, 1964.Google Scholar
  73. Stein, L., Wise, C. D., & Berger, B. D. Anti-anxiety action of benzodiazepines: Decrease in activity of serotonin neurons in the punishment system. In S. Garattini, E. Mussini, & L. O. Randall (Eds.), The benzodiazepines. New York: Raven Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  74. Stumpf, Ch. Drug action on the electrical activity of the hippocampus. International Review of Neurobiology, 1965, 8, 77–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Sutherland, N. S. Partial reinforcement and breadth of learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 18, 289–301.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Ungerstedt, U. 6-Hydroxydopamine induced degeneration of central monoamine neurons. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1968, 5, 107–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Ungerstedt, u. Stereotaxic mapping of the monoamine pathways in the rat brain. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 1971, 82 (Suppl. 367), 1–48.Google Scholar
  78. Vanderwolf, C. H., Kramis, R., Gillespie, L. A. & Bland, B. H. Hippocampal rhythmical slow activity and neocortical low voltage fast activity: relations to behavior. In R. L. Isaacson & K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The hippocampus, Vol. 2. New York: Plenum, 1975, pp. 101–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wagner, A. R. Sodium amytal and partially reinforced runway performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 65, 474–477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Wagner, A. R. Frustration and punishment. In R. M. Haber (Ed.), Current research on motivation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966.Google Scholar
  81. Weiskrantz, L. Emotion. In L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Analysis of behavioral change. New York: Harper & Row, 1968.Google Scholar
  82. Weiss, J. M., Glazer, H. I., Pohorecky, L. A., Brick, J., & Miller, N. E. Effects of chronic exposure to stressors on avoidance-escape behavior and on brain norepinephrine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1975, 37, 522–534.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Ziff, P. R., & Capaldi, E. J. Amytal and the small trial partial reinforcement effect: Stimulus properties of early trial nonrewards. Journal of Experimental Pscychology, 1971, 87, 263–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1979

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. A. Gray
    • 1
  1. 1.University of OxfordOxfordEngland

Personalised recommendations