Skip to main content

Starlings and Optimal Foraging Theory: Modelling in a Fractal World

  • Chapter
Book cover Foraging Behavior

Abstract

Whether the optimality approach to the study of behavior is a respectable scientific theory has often been the subject of debate (Maynard Smith 1978; Gould & Lewontin 1979; Gray this volume; Ollason 1980 and this volume). The argument is partly obscured by the semantic difficulties associated with the use of the term “theory” and its difference from “model” and “hypothesis.” Behavioral ecologists must be concerned with this debate because it is important to establish the relationship between theoretical statements and empirical evidence and to be aware of what is being tested when biological data are compared with theoretical predictions. It is also important to determine the relative advantages of alternative research programs. Differences in outlook between authors belonging to various schools of behavioral research are common. While one occasionally reads statements such as “optimality theory predicts (or fails to predict) the observed results,” thus implying that it is the theory itself that is under experimental test (Mazur 1981; Diamond 1984; Fantino & Abarca 1985; Mellgren & Brown in press; Rashotte in press), evolutionary zoologists interested in behavior usually maintain that optimality itself is not being tested, but rather that specific models derived from the theory are (Maynard Smith 1978; Curio 1983; Kacelnik 1984; Cheverton, Kacelnik & Krebs 1985; Schmid-Hempel, Kacelnik & Houston 1985).

“Consider for instance one of the white flakes obtained by salting a solution of soap. At a distance its contours may appear sharply defined, but as we draw nearer its sharpness disappears… The use of a magnifying glass or microscope leaves us just as uncertain, for fresh irregularities appear every time we increase the magnification, and we never succeed in getting a sharp smooth impression…” Perrin J. 1906.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Brooke, M. de L. 1981. How an adult wheatear (Oenanthe oenanthe) uses its territory when feeding nestlings. Journal of Animal Ecology, 50: 683–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, D., and Turner, A.K. 1982. Central place foraging by swallows (Hirundinidae): the question of load size. Animal Behaviour, 30: 845–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caraco, T., and Lima, S.L. 1986. Survival, Energy Budgets and Foraging risks. In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Volume 6: Foraging (ed. by M. Commons, A. Kacelnik, & S. Shettleworth), in press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, A., and Moreno, J. 1981. Central place foraging in the wheatear, Oenanthe oenanthe: an experimental test. Journal of Animal Ecology, 50: 917–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E.L. 1976. Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9: 129–136.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cheverton, J., Kacelnik, A., and Krebs, J.R. 1985. Optimal Foraging: constraints and currencies. In: Experimental Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (ed. by B. Holldobler, & M. Lindauer), 31:109–126. Fortschritte der Zoologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowie, R.J. 1977. Optimal foraging in Great Tits (Parus major). Nature, 268: 137–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curio, E. 1983. Time-energy budgets and optimization. Experientia, 39: 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuthill, I.C. 1985. Experimental Studies of Optimal Foraging Theory. Unpublished D. Philosophy Thesis, University of Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, P. 1984. Optimal foraging theory tested. Nature, 311: 603–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duhem, P. 1914. La theorie physique, son object, sa structure. 2nd edition. Paris: M. Riviere & Cie. (English translation: 1954. The Aim and Structure of Physical Theory. Princeton University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Emlen, J.M. 1966. The role of time and energy in food preference. American Naturalist, 100: 611–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., and Abarca, N. 1985. Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. Behavior and Brain Science, 8: 315–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L., and Kramer, D.L. 1982. The marginal value theorem: a quantitative test using load size variation in a central place forager, the eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Animal Behaviour, 30: 1036–1042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S.J., and Lewontin, R.C. 1979. The Spandrels of San Marco and the panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Procedures of R. Soc. Biology, 205: 581–598.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Green, R.F. 1979. Bayesian Birds: A Simple Example of Paten’s Stochastic Model of Optimal Foraging. University of California at Riverside, Technical Report 50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, R.F. 1984. Stopping rules for optimal foragers. American Naturalist, 123: 30–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, C.B. 1981. Learning the evolutionary stable strategy. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 89: 611–633.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Houston, A.I., Kacelnik, A., and McNamara, J. 1982. Some learning rules for acquiring information. In: Functional Ontogeny (ed. by D.J. McFarland ), pp. 140–191. Pitman, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A. 1984. Central place foraging in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). I. Patch residence time. Journal of Animal Ecology, 53: 283–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A., and Houston, A.I. 1984. Some effects of energy costs on foraging strategies. Animal Behaviour, 32: 609–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A.,’and Krebs, J.R. 1985. Learning about food distribution. In: Behavioural Ecology: Ecological Consequences of Adaptive Behaviour (ed. by R.M. Sibly & R. Smith ). Blackwell’s Scientific Publications, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A., Krebs, J.R., and Ens, B. 1986. Foraging in a changing environment: An experiment with starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Volume 6: Foraging (ed. by M. Commons. A. Kacelnik, & S. Shettleworth), in press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A. 1986. Short term adjustments of parental effort in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Proceedings of the XIX International Ornithological Conference, Ottawa 1986. In press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, D.L., and Nowell, W. 1980. Central place foraging in the eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus. Animal Behaviour, 28: 772–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J.R., Kacelnik, A., and Taylor, P. 1978. Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature, 275: 27–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T.S. 1970. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd Edition. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. 1970. Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In: Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge (ed. Lakatos, I ), pp. 91–195. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester, N.P. 1984. The feed: feed decision: how goldfish solve the patch depletion problem. Behaviour, 89: 175–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S.L. 1984. Downy woodpecker foraging behavior: efficient sampling in simple stochastic environments. Ecology, 65: 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S.L. 1985. Sampling behavior of starlings foraging in simple patchy environments. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 16: 135–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, R.M., and Pianka, E.R. 1966. On optimal use of a patchy environment. American Naturalist, 100: 603–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandelbrot, B.B. 1983. The Fractal Geometry of Nature. W.H. Freeman & Co., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard-Smith, J. 1978. Optimization Theory in Evolution. Annual Review of Ecology Systematics, 9: 31–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, J.M. 1982. Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment. Theoretical Population Biology, 21: 269–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J.M. 1981. Optimization theory fails to predict performance of pigeons in a two-response situation. Science, 214: 823–824.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mellgren, R., and Brown, S.W. Environmental Constraints on Optimal Foraging Behavior. In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Volume 6: Foraging (ed. by M. Commons, A. Kacelnik, & S. Shettleworth), in press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milinski, M. 1984. Competitive resource sharing: an experimental test of a learning rule for ESSs. Animal Behaviour, 32: 233–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norberg, R.A. 1981. Temporary weight decrease in breeding birds may result in more fledged young. American Naturalist, 118: 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaten, S.A. 1977. Optimal foraging in patches: a case for stochasticity. Theoretical Population Biology, 12: 263–285.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ollason, J.G. 1980. Learning to forage-optimally? Theoretical Population Biology, 18: 44–56.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G.H., and Pearson, N.E. 1979. On the theory of central place foraging. In: Analysis of Ecological Systems (ed. by D.J. Horn, R.D. Mitchell, & G.R. Stairs ), pp. 154–177. Columbus: Ohio State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrin, J. 1906. La discontinuité de la matiere. Translated by B.B. Mandelbrot, 1983. Revue du mois, 1: 323–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rashotte, M.E., O’Connell, J.M., and Djuric, V.J. 1986. Mechanisms of signal-controlled foraging behavior. In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Volume 6: Foraging (ed. by M. Commons, A. Kacelnik, & S. Shettleworth), in press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmid-Hempel, P., Kacelnik, A., and Houston, A.I. 1985. Honeybees maximize efficiency by not filling their crops. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 17: 61–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoener, T.W. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 2: 369–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S.J. 1986. Learning and foraging in pigeons. In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior, Volume 6: Foraging (ed. by M. Commons, A. Kacelnik, & S. Shettleworth), in press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, J.M. 1981. Foraging decisions in starlings. Ardea, 69.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1987 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kacelink, A., Cuthill, I.C. (1987). Starlings and Optimal Foraging Theory: Modelling in a Fractal World. In: Kamil, A.C., Krebs, J.R., Pulliam, H.R. (eds) Foraging Behavior. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-1839-2_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-1839-2_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-9027-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-1839-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics