Looking for Introspection; Self-Consciousness, Self-Awareness, and Emotionality

Preliminary Empirical Findings
  • David Bromberg
  • Lorne M. Hartman
Part of the Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect book series (ASCA, volume 11)

Abstract

When science looks back and evaluates the relative importance of a research document, one of the yardsticks used is the amount of research and, occasionally, the manner of controversy that follows the publication. By this definition, Schachter and Singer’s (1962) study must be regarded as a classic in psychology. The paper set a course for the study of the role of cognition in emotion (e.g., Lazarus, 1968; Nowlis, 1970; Valins, 1970) and kindled a dispute that was furthered by Schachter’s (1964) subsequent conclusion that an emotional event1 occurs when an ambiguous state of physiological arousal is combined with an appropriate cognition. Based on this conclusion and a methodology relying on misdirection, misinformation, and deception, a premise was established suggesting that internal states are ambiguous and uninterpretable without appropriate context. Consequently, without context, a feeling could not be matched to an affective label. This theoretical notion implies that introspective awareness is not a viable phenomenon and is the foundation of the ensuing controversy.

Keywords

Depression Covariance Guaran Glean 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74 ,183–200.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bem, D. J. (1970). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62.). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  3. Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  4. Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (1979). Focus of attention, chronic expectancy, and responses to a feared stimulus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 ,1186–1195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Duval, S. (1976). Conformity on a visual task as a function of personal novelty on attitudinal dimensions and being reminded of the object status of self. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12 ,87–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  7. Exner, J. E. (1973). The self focus sentence completion: A study of egocentricity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 37 ,437–455.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fenigstein, A. (1979). Self-consciousness, self-attention, and social interaction. Journal of Person ality and Social Psychology, 37 ,75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assesment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43 ,522–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gibbons, F. X. (1978). Sexual standards and reactions to pornography: Enhancing behavioral consistency through self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 ,976–987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Goffman, E. (1956). The presentation of self in everyday life. Edinburgh, Scotland: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
  12. Kuiken, D., Neuhaus, D., & Bromberg, D. (1982). The effects of private and public self-consciousness on the expression of an emotional experience. Unpublished research document.Google Scholar
  13. Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: Conceptual and empirical relations. In M. B. Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  14. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  15. Nisbett, R. E., & Bellows, N. (1977). Verbal reports about causal influences on social judgements: Private access versus public theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 ,613– 624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Nisbett, R. E., & Schachter, S. (1966). Cognitive manipulation of pain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2 ,227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84 ,231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Nowlis, V. (1970). Mood: Behavior and experience. In M. B. Arnold (Ed.), Feelings and emotions: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 261–277). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Polivy, J. (1981). On the induction of emotion in the laboratory: Discrete moods or multiple affect states? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 ,803–817.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in experimental social psychology (Volume 1, pp. 49–80). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69 ,378–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Scheier, M. F. (1976). Self-awareness, self-consciousness and angry aggression. Journal of Personality, 44 ,627–644.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1977). Self-focused attention and the experience of emotion: Attraction, repulsion, elation and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35 ,625–636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Scheier, M. F., Buss, A. H., & Buss, D. M. (1978). Self-consciousness, self-report of aggressiveness, and aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 12 ,133–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Smith, E. R., & Miller, F. D. (1978). Limits on perception of cognitive processes: A reply to Nisbett and Wilson. Psychological Review, 85 ,355–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30 ,526–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Valins, S. (1970). The perception and labelling of bodily changes as determinants of emotional behavior. In P. Black (Ed.) Physiological correlates of emotion (pp. 229–243). New York: Academic Press,Google Scholar
  28. Valins, S., & Ray, A. A. (1967). Effects of cognitive desensitization on avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 ,345–350.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 233–275). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  30. Zimbardo, P. G., Cohen, A., Weisenberg, M., Dworkin, L., & Firestone, I. (1969). The control of experimental pain. In P. G. Zimbardo (Ed.), The cognitive control of motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
  31. Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965). Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Bromberg
    • 1
  • Lorne M. Hartman
    • 2
  1. 1.Graduate School of Education and Human DevelopmentUniversity of RochesterRochesterUSA
  2. 2.Addiction Research FoundationUniversity of TorontoTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations