Advertisement

The Development of Attribution Processes

  • Saul M. Kassin
  • John B. Pryor

Abstract

Analyses of naive causal explanations have a long and venerable history in both social and developmental psychology (Heider, 1944; Piaget, 1930). Heider (1944) postulated that any “change in the environment gains its meaning from the source to which it is attributed” (p. 372). This “inferred meaning” was for Heider the wellspring for all social behavior. Piaget was similarly impressed with the pivotal role of causal explantion in the development of children’s thinking. Indeed it was Piaget’s view that “the child’s basic orientation towards causal explantion intrudes throughout the length and breadth of his cognitive life” (Flavell, 1963, p. 284).

Keywords

Intrinsic Motivation Causal Attribution Experimental Social Psychology Causal Judgment Perceptual Salience 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abelson, R. P. (1976). Script processing in attitude formation and decision-making. In J. S. Carroll & J. W. Payne (Eds.), Cognition and social behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–729Google Scholar
  3. Ausubel, D. P., & Schiff, H. M. (1954), The effect of incidental and experimentally induced experience in the learning of relevant and irrelevant causal relationships by children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 84, 109–123PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Ball, W. A. (1973). The perception of causality in the infant. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  5. Banks, W. C. (1976). White preference in blacks: A paradigm in search of a phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1176–1186.Google Scholar
  6. Bassili, J. N. (1976). Temporal and spatial contingencies in the perception of social events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 680–685.Google Scholar
  7. Berndt, T. J. (1977). The effect of reciprocity norms on moral judgment and causal attribution. Child Development, 48, 1322–1330.Google Scholar
  8. Berzonsky, M. (1971). The role of familiarity in children’s explanations of physical causality. Child Development, 42, 705–715.Google Scholar
  9. Blumenfeld, P., Bossert, S., Hamilton, U., Wesslo, C., & Mecce, J. (1979). Teacher talk and student thought: Socialization to the student role. Paper presented at the Conference on Teacher and Student Perceptions of Success and Failure, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
  10. Borton, R. W. (1979). The perception of causality in infants. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  11. Bovee, G. R. (1985). The attributional and memorial consequences of processing covariation information. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN.Google Scholar
  12. Brainerd, C. J. (1973). Judgments and explanations on criteria for the presence of cognitive structures. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 172–179.Google Scholar
  13. Brainerd, C. J. (1977). Response criteria in concept development. Child Development, 48, 360–366.Google Scholar
  14. Bruner, J. (1966). On the conservation of liquids. In J. S. Bruner, R. Oliver, & P. Greenfield (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Bullock, M., & Gelman, R. (1979). Preschool children’s assumptions about cause and effect: Temporal ordering. Child Development, 50, 89–96.Google Scholar
  16. Butzin, V. (1979). Children’s moral judgments of ulterior motives. Paper presented at the Biennial Convention of the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  17. Carni, E., & French, L. (1984). The acquisition of before and after reconsidered: What develops? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 394–403.Google Scholar
  18. Case, R. (1978). Intellectual development from birth to adulthood: A neo-Piagetian interpretation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Cohen, E. A., Gelfand, D. M., & Hartmann, D. P. (1981). Causal reasoning as a function of behavioral consequences. Child Development, 52, 514–522.Google Scholar
  20. Costanzo, P., & Dix, T. (1983). Beyond the information processed: Socialization and the development of attribution processes. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Costanzo, P., Goie, J., Grumet, J., & Farnill, D. (1973). A re-examination of the effects of intent and consequence on children’s moral judgments. Child Development, 44, 154–161.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Costanzo, P., Grumet, J., & Brehm, S. (1974). The effects of choice and source of constraint on children’s attributions of preference. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 352–364.Google Scholar
  23. Crocker, J. (1981). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psychological Bulletin, 9, 272–292.Google Scholar
  24. Dalenberg, C. J., Bierman, K. L., & Furman, W. (1984). A reexamination of developmental changes in causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 20, 575–583.Google Scholar
  25. D’Andrade, R. G. (1981). The cultural part of cognition. Cognitive Science, 5, 179–195.Google Scholar
  26. DiVitto, B., & McArthur, L. Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness, consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 474–482.Google Scholar
  27. Dix, T., & Herzberger, S. (1983). The role of logic and salience in the development of causal attribution. Child Development, 54, 960–967.Google Scholar
  28. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, (Whole No. 270).Google Scholar
  29. Farnill, D. (1974). The effects of social judgment set on children’s use of intent information. Journal of Personality, 42, 276–284.Google Scholar
  30. Fazio, R. H. (1981). On the self-perception explanation of the overjustification effect: The role of salience of initial attitude. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 417–426.Google Scholar
  31. Fineham, F. D. (1983). Developmental dimensions of attribution theory. In J. Jaspars, F. Fineham, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Attribution theory and research: Developmental and social dimensions. London: Academic.Google Scholar
  32. Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
  33. Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Grumet, J. (1975). Effects of adults’ and peer sanctions on children’s attributions of preference. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Duke University, Durham, NC.Google Scholar
  35. Harvey, J. H., Town, J. P., & Yarkin, K. L. (1981). How fundamental is “the fundamental attribution error”? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 346–349.Google Scholar
  36. Heider, F. (1958) The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley Google Scholar
  37. Higgins, E. T., & Bryant, S. L. (1982). Consensus information and the fundamental attribution error: The role of development and in-group versus out-group knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 889–900.Google Scholar
  38. Higgins, E. T., & Parsons, J. E. (1983). Social cognition and the social life of the child: Stages as subcultures. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Higgins, E. T., Ruble, D. N., & Hartup, W. W. (1983). Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Hume, D. (1960). A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Clarendon (Originally published, 1740 ).Google Scholar
  41. Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  42. Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24.Google Scholar
  43. Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. Kelley, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
  44. Kahneman, D. Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Kant, I. (1961). Kritik der reinen vernunft. In W. Kaufmann (Ed.), Philosophic classics:Vol. 2. Bacon to Kant. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall (Original work published 1781 ).Google Scholar
  46. Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1976). The development of causal attributions in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 455–464.Google Scholar
  47. Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1979). Children’s use of a causal attribution schema and the influence of manipulative intentions. Child Development, 50, 463–468.Google Scholar
  48. Kassin, S. M. (1981). From “laychild” to “layman.” Developmental causal attribution. In S. Brehm, S. Kassin, & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Developmental social psychology: Theory and research. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Kassin, S. M., & Baron, R. M. (1985). Basic determinants of attribution and social perception. In J. Harvey & G. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (Vol. 1 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  50. Kassin, S. M., & Gibbons, F. X. (1981). Children’s use of the discounting principle in their perceptions of exertion. Child Development, 52, 71–744.Google Scholar
  51. Kassin, S. M., & Lepper, M. R. (1984). Oversufficient and insufficient justification effects: Cognitive and behavioral development. In J. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
  52. Kassin, S. M., & Lowe, C. A. (1979). On the development of the augmentation principle: A perceptual approach. Child Development, 50, 728–734.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Kassin, S. M., Lowe, C. A., & Gibbons, F. X. (1983). Children’s use of the discounting principle: A perceptual approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 718–728.Google Scholar
  54. Kaye, H. (1967). Infant sucking behavior and its modification. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. C. Spiker (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, (Vol. 3 ). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  55. Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  56. Kelley, H. H. (1971). Causal schemata and the attribution process. Morristown, NJ: General Learning.Google Scholar
  57. Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.Google Scholar
  58. Kessen, W. (1979). The American child and other cultural inventions. American Psychologist, 34, 815–820.Google Scholar
  59. Kimble, G. A. (1947). Conditioning as a function of the time between conditioned and unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 1–15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestaltpsychology. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  61. Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientation toward a moral order: 1. Sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11–33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Kruglanski, A. W., Alon, S., & Lewis, T. (1972). Retrospective misattribution and task enjoyment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 493–501.Google Scholar
  63. Kruglanski, A. W., Schwartz, J. M., Maides, S., & Hamel, I. Z. (1978). Covariation, discounting, and augmentation: Towards a clarification of attribution principles. Journal of Personality, 46, 176–189.Google Scholar
  64. Kuhn, D., & Phelps, H. (1976). The development of children’s comprehension of causal direction. Child Development, 47, 248–251.Google Scholar
  65. Kun, A. (1977). Development of the magnitude-covariation and compensation schemata in ability and effort attributions of performance. Child Development, 48, 862–873.Google Scholar
  66. Kun, A. (1978). Evidence for preschoolers’ understanding of causal direction in extended causal sequences. Child Development, 49, 218–222.Google Scholar
  67. Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (1979). Cross-cultural psychology’s challenges to our ideas of children and development. American Psychologist, 34, 827–832.Google Scholar
  68. Leahy, R. L. (1979). Development of conceptions of prosocial behavior: Information affecting rewards given for altruism and kindness. Developmental Psychology, 15, 34–37.Google Scholar
  69. Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the “overjustiflcation” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.Google Scholar
  70. Lepper, M. R., Sagotsky, G., Dafoe, J., & Greene, D. (1982). Consequences of superfluous social constraints: Effects of nominal contingencies on children’s subsequent intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 51–65.Google Scholar
  71. Leslie, A. M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants, Perception, 11 173–186.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Lesser, H. (1977). The growth of perceived causality in children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 145–152.Google Scholar
  73. Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  74. Lipsitt, L. P., & Kaye, H. (1964). Conditioned sucking in the human newborn. Psychonomic Science, 2, 29–30.Google Scholar
  75. Livesley, W., & Bromley, D. (1973). Person perception in childhood and adolescence. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  76. McArthur, L. A. (1972). The how and what of why: Some determinants and consequences of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 171–193.Google Scholar
  77. McArthur, L. Z., & Post, D. L. S. (1977). Figural emphasis on person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 520–535.Google Scholar
  78. Mead, M. (1962). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: Morrow.Google Scholar
  79. Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  80. Mendelson, R. & Shultz, T. R. (1976). Covariation and temporal contiguity as principles of causal inference in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 22, 408–412.Google Scholar
  81. Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. London: Metheun (Original work published in 1946 ).Google Scholar
  82. Morgan, M. (1983). Decrements in intrinsic motivation among rewarded and observer subjects. Child Development, 54, 636–644.Google Scholar
  83. Much, N. C. & Schweder, R. A. (1978). Speaking of rules: The analysis of culture in the breach. In W. Damon (Ed.), New directions for child development, Vol. 2: Moral development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  84. Nelson, K. (1981). Social cognition in a script framework. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  85. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know verbal on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.Google Scholar
  86. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  87. Odom, R. D., Astor, E. C., & Cunningham, J. G. (1975). Effects of perceptual salience on the matrix task performance of four and six-year-old children. Child Development, 46, 757–762.Google Scholar
  88. Olum, V. (1956). Developmental differences in the perception of causality. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 417–428.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Orvis, B. R., Cunningham, J. D., & Kelley, H. H. (1975). A closer examination of causal inference: The roles of consensus, distinctiveness, and consistency information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 605–616.Google Scholar
  90. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  91. Peevers, B., & Secord, P. (1973). Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 120–128.Google Scholar
  92. Piaget, J. (1930). The child’s conception of physical causality. London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  93. Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  94. Piaget, J. (1963/1932) The moral judgment of the child New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  95. Piaget, J. (1974). Understanding causality. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  96. Pool, D. L., Shweder, R., & Much, N. (1983). Culture as a cognitive system: Differentiated role understanding in children and other savages. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  97. Pryor, J.B., & Kriss, M. (1977). The cognitive dynamics of salience in the attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35, 49–55.Google Scholar
  98. Pryor, J. B., Rholes, W. S., Ruble, D. M., & Kriss, M. (In Press). A developmental analysis of salience and discounting in social attribution. Representative Research in Social Psychology Google Scholar
  99. Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1970). Experimental child psychology. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  100. Renner, K. E. (1964). Delay of reinforcement: An historical overview. Psychological Bulletin, 61, 341–361.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. M. Black & W. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  102. Rholes, W. S. & Ruble, D. N. (1984). Children’s understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.Google Scholar
  103. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  104. Ross, M. (1975). Salience of reward and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 245–254.Google Scholar
  105. Ruble, D. M., Feldmaft, N. S., Higgins, E. T., & Karlovac, M. (1979). Locus of causality and the use of information in the development of causal attributions. Journal of Personality, 47, 595–614.Google Scholar
  106. Salili, F., Maehr, M., & Gilmore, G. (1976). Achievement and morality: A cross- cultural analysis of attribution and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 327–337.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Schank, R. & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  108. Schwartz, T. (1981). The acquisition of culture. Ethos, 9, 4–17.Google Scholar
  109. Sedlak, A. J., & Kurtz, S. T. (1981). A review of children’s use of causal inference principles. Child Development, 52, 759–784.Google Scholar
  110. Shaklee, H. (1976). Development in inferences of ability and task difficulty. Child Development, 47, 1051–1057.Google Scholar
  111. Shaklee, H., & Tucker, D. (1979). Cognitive bases of development in inferences of ability. Child Development, 50, pp. 904–907.Google Scholar
  112. Sharp, K. C. (1982). Preschoolers’ understanding of temporal and causal relations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 427–436.Google Scholar
  113. Shultz, T. R. (1982). Causal reasoning in the social and nonsocial realms. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 14, 307–322.Google Scholar
  114. Shultz, T. R., & Butkowski, I. (1977). Young children’s use of the scheme for multiple sufficient causes in the attribution of real and hypothetical behavior. Child Development, 48, 464–469.Google Scholar
  115. Shultz, T. R., Butkowsky, I., Pearce, J. W., & Shanfield, H. (1975). Development of schemes for the attribution of multiple psychological causes. Developmental Psychology, 11, 502–510.Google Scholar
  116. Shultz, T. R., & Mendelson, R. (1975). The use of covariation as a principle of causal analysis: Child Development, 46, 394–399.Google Scholar
  117. Shultz, T. R, & Ravinsky, F. B. (1977). Similarity as a principle of causal inference. Child Development, 48, 1552–1558.Google Scholar
  118. Siegler, R. S. (1975). Defining the locus of developmental differences in children’s causal reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 512–525.Google Scholar
  119. Siegler, R S., & Liebert, R. M. (1974). Effects of contiguity, regularity, and age on children’s causal inferences. Developmental Psychology, 10, 574–579.Google Scholar
  120. Smith, M. C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.Google Scholar
  121. Smith, M. C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.Google Scholar
  122. Taylor, S. E., Crocker, J., Fiske, S. T., Sprinzen, M., & Winkler, J. D. (1979). The generalizability of saliene effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 357–368.Google Scholar
  123. Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439–445.Google Scholar
  124. Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture: Researchers into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art and custom. London: Murray Google Scholar
  125. Wells, D., & Shultz, T. R (1980). Developmental distinctions between behavior and judgment in the operation of the discounting principle. Child Development, 51, 1307–1310.Google Scholar
  126. Wells, G. L. (1980). Lay analyses of causal forces on behavior. In J. H. Harvey (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior and the environment. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  127. Werner, H. & Kaplan, E. (1956). The developmental approach to cognition: Its relevance to the psychological interpretive of anthropological and ethnolinguistie data. American Anthropologist, 58, 866–880.Google Scholar
  128. White, P. (1984). A model of the layperson as pragmatist. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 333–348.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Saul M. Kassin
  • John B. Pryor

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations