Crowd Mind and Behavior: Afterthoughts

  • Carl F. Graumann
Part of the Springer Series in Social Psychology book series (SSSOC)

Abstract

A discussion of the major conceptions of crowd mind and behavior, of mass society, and of the phenomena of crowding is bound to reiterate certain topics of interest which have come up in the ongoing controversies about mass phenomena, their description and, above all, their explanation. This is true for both the frequent intra- disciplinary and the rare interdisciplinary dialogue on masses. It, therefore, is not surprising that in the preceding chapters we find such recurrent themes, whether the chapter was written from an historical, sociological, or psychological perspective. Leaning on the contributions as well as on the very lively discussion in the study group, I shall try to concentrate on some of these themes. With a view to sharpening the issues I shall treat them as bipolar dimensions of the mass behavior analysis. In some cases, the dimensional character may still be a euphemism, since dichotomy prevails. Considering, however, that it has been the goal of the study group to incorporate conceptions of historical change, which should include historical conceptions of change, into social science (here, of mass phenomena) I think it wiser to fixate on the goal of our interdisciplinary enterprise rather than on the much-criticized status quo. Still, I prefer to label the heuristic dimensions by their poles: (a) historical versus scientific approaches, (b) top-down versus bottom-up conceptions, (c) individual versus crowd-centered approaches, and (d) rationalist versus inationalist explanations of crowd mind and behavior. They all turn out to be questions.

Keywords

Stratification Lewin 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allport, G. W. (1968). The historical background of modern social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 1–80 ). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  2. Barker, R., Dembo, T., & Lewin, K. (1941). Frustration and regression. University of Iowa Google Scholar
  3. Studies in Child Welfare, 18 (1).Google Scholar
  4. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: Murray. Dipboye, R. L. (1977). Alternative approaches to deindividuation. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 1057–1075.Google Scholar
  5. Festinger, L. (1983). The human legacy. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Festinger, L., Pepitone, A., & Newcomb, T. (1952). Some consequences of deindividuationin a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 382–389.Google Scholar
  7. Freud, S. (1953). Totem and taboo. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. XIII ). London: Hogarth Press. ( Original work published 1913 )Google Scholar
  8. Freud, S. (1953). Metaphysical supplement to the theory of dreams. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. XIV ). London: Hogarth Press. ( Original work published 1916 )Google Scholar
  9. Freud, S. (1953). Group psychology and the analysis of the ego. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. XVIII). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1921 )Google Scholar
  10. Haeckel, E. (1874). Anthropologie. Entwicklungsgeschichte des menschen. Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
  11. Hall, G. St. (1904). Adolescence. Its psychology and its relation to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion and education. New York: D. Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink. A psychological study of foreign policy decisions and fiascoes. Boston: Houghton & Mifflin.Google Scholar
  13. Klix, F. (1980). Erwachendes Denken. Eine Entwicklungsgeschichte der menschlichen Intelligenz. Berlin: VEB Verlag der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
  14. Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
  15. Le Bon, G. (1895). Psychologie des foules. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
  16. Lewin, K. (1951). In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  17. Pruitt, D. G. (1971). Choice shifts in group discussion: An introductory review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20, 339–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Stern, C., & Stern, W. (1907). Die Kindersprache. Eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung. Leipzig: J. A. Barth.Google Scholar
  19. Werner, H. (1957). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  20. Woodward, W., & Ash, M. (Eds.). (1982). The problematic science: Psychology in nineteenth-century thought. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  21. Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The human choice. Individuation, reason and order versus deindividuation, impulse and chaos. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 17, pp. 237 - 307 ). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Carl F. Graumann

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations