Patterns of Lateralized Behavior in Prosimians

  • Jeannette P. Ward
  • Garrett W. Milliken
  • Donna K. Stafford
Part of the Recent Research in Psychology book series (PSYCHOLOGY)

Abstract

In the preceding chapter, Harris has thoroughly documented the enduring interest of the human species in the question of lateral asymmetry in the behavior of nonhuman primate species. The appearance of this book with reports of a variety of nonhuman primate laterality studies originating in many parts of the world testifies to the resurgence of interest in this topic. This chapter reports the incidence and patterning of laterality phenomena in species of prosimians, a major group of nonhuman primates much less studied in all research contexts than their anthropoid counterparts. The relative neglect of these most primitive of primates, which represent the stem from which the primate order originated, has been undergoing remediation and there now exist many sources of information about this group (see for example, Charles-Dominique, 1977; Charles-Dominiqueet al., 1980; Doyle,1974; Doyle & Martin, 1979; Jolly, 1966; Martin, 1990; Martin, Doyle, & Walker, 1974; Tattersall, 1982).

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Annett, M. (1972). The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 343–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Annett, M. (1978). Genetic and nongenetic influences on handedness. Behavior Genetics, 8, 227–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bard, K.A., Hopkins, W.D., & Fort, C.L. (1990). Lateral bias in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 309–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bearder, S.K., & Doyle, G.A. (1974). Ecology of bushbabies, G. senegalensis and G. crassicaudatus, with some notes about their behaviour in the field. In R.D. Martin, G.A. Doyle, & A. C. Walker (Eds.), Prosimian behaviour (pp. 109–130). London: Duckworth Press.Google Scholar
  5. Charles-Dominique, P. (1977). Ecology and behaviour of nocturnal primates. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Charles-Dominique, P., Cooper, H.M., Hladik, A., Hladik, C.M., Pages, E., Pariente, G.F., Petter-Rousseaux, A., Petter, J.J., & Schilling, A. (Eds.) (1980). Nocturnal malagasy primates. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  7. Collins, R.L. (1969). On the inheritance of handedness. II. Selection for sinistrality in mice. Journal of Heredity, 60, 117–119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, R.L. (1975). When left-handed mice live in right-handed worlds. Science, 187, 181–184.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Covert, H.H., & Kay, R.F. (1981). Dental microwear and diet: Implications for determining the feeding behaviors of extinct primates, with a comment on the dietary pattern of Sivapithecus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 55, 331–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dart, R.A. (1949). The predatory implemental technique of Australopithecus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 7, 1–38.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dodson, D.L., Leduc, P., Larson, C.F., & Ward, J.P. (1992). Left hand advantage in reaching in the galago (Galago moholi). Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  12. Dodson, D.L., Stafford, D.K., Forsythe, C, Seltzer, C.P. & Ward, J.P. (1992). Laterality in quadrapedal and bipedal prosimians: Reach and whole-body turn in the mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus) and the galago (Galago moholi). American Journal of Primatology, 26, 191–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Doyle, G.A. (1974). Behavior of prosimians. In A.M. Schrier & F. Stollnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (pp. 155–353). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  14. Doyle, G.A., & Martin, R.D. (1979). The study of prosimian behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Forsythe, C., Milliken, G.W., Stafford, D.K., & Ward, J.P. (1988). Posturally related variations in the hand preferences of the ruffed lemur (Varecia variegata variegata). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 102, 248–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Forsythe, C., & Ward, J.P. (1988). Black lemur (Lemur macaco) hand preference in food reaching. Primates, 29, 369–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fortuyn, J.D. (1982). On the organization of action: A new synthesis. Human Neurobiology, 1, 145–151.Google Scholar
  18. Frost, T.F. (1980). Tool behavior and the origins of laterality. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 447–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Geschwind, N. (1985). Implications for evolution, genetics, and clinical syndromes. In S.D.Glick (Ed.), Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species (pp.247–278). Orlando, Florida: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A.M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization. Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: I. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 428–459.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A.M. (1987). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  22. Glick, S.D. (1983). Cerebral lateralization in the rat and tentative extrapolations to man. In M.S.Myslobodsky (Ed.), Hemisyndromes: Psychobiology, neurology, psychiatry (pp.7–26). Orlando, Florida: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  23. Glick, S.D., & Ross, D.A. (1981). Lateralized effects of bilateral frontal cortex lesions in rats. Brain Research, 210, 379–382.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Glick, S.D., & Shapiro, R.M. (1985). Functional and neurochemical mechanisms of cerebral lateralization in rats. In S.D. Glick (Ed.), Cerebral lateralization in nonhuman species (pp. 157–183). Orlando, Florida: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  25. Hamilton, C.R. & Vermeire, B.A. (1988). Complementary hemispheric specialization in monkeys. Science, 242, 1691–1694.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hamilton, C.R., & Vermeire, B.A. (1991). Functional lateralization in monkeys. In F.L. Kitterle (Ed.) Cerebral laterality: Theory and research (pp. 19–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Hefiher, H.E., & Hefïher, R.S. (1984). Temporal lobe lesions and perception of species-specific vocalizations by macaques. Science, 226, 75–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Heffner, H.E., & Heffner, R.S. (1986). Effect of unilateral and bilateral auditory cortex lesions on the discriminations of Japanese macaques. Journal of Neurophysiology, 56, 683–701.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Hopkins, W.D., & Morris, R.D. (1989). Laterality for visual-spatial processing in two language-trained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 227–234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hopkins, W.D., Washburn, D.A., & Rumbaugh, D.M. (1990). Processing of form stimuli presented unilaterally in humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and monkeys (Macaca mulatto). Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 577–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur behavior. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Jouffroy, F.K. (1962). La musculature des membres chez les Lemuriens de Madagascar. Etude descriptive et comparative. Mammalia, 55, 1–326.Google Scholar
  33. Jouffroy, F.K (1975). Osteology and myology of the lemuriform postcranial skeleton. In I. Tattersall & R.W. Sussman (Eds.), Lemur biology (pp. 149–192). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  34. Kay, R.F. (1984). On the use of anatomical features to infer foraging behavior in extinct primates. In P.S. Rodman & J.G.H. Cant (Eds.) Adaptations for foraging in nonhuman primates (pp.21–53). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Kay, R.F. & Hylander, W.L. (1978). The dental structure of mammalian folivores with special reference to Primates and Phalangeroidea (Marsupialia). In G.G. Montgomery (Ed.), The ecology of arboreal folivores (pp. 173–196). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
  36. Larson, C.F., Dodson, D., & Ward, J.P. (1989). Hand preferences and whole-body turning biases of lesser bushbabies (Galago senegalensis). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 33, 261–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. MacNeilage, P.F. (1991). The postural origins theory of neurobiological asymmetries in primates. In N. Krasnegor, D. Rumbuagh, M.G. Studdert-Kennedy, & R. Schiefelbusch (Eds.), The biological foundations of language development (pp. 165–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. MacNeilage, P.F., Studdert-Kennedy, M.G., & Lindblom, B. (1987). Primate handedness reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 247–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. MacNeilage, P.F., Studdert-Kennedy, M.G., & Lindblom, B. (1988). Primate handedness: A foot in the door. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 737–744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. MacNeilage, P.F., Studdert-Kennedy, M.G., & Lindblom, B. (1991). Primate handedness: The other theory, the other hand and the other attitude. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 344–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Martin, R.D. (1990). Primate origins and evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Martin, R.D., Doyle, G.A., & Walker, A.C. (1974). Prosimian biology. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
  43. Masataka, N. (1989). Population-level asymmetry of hand preference in lemurs. Behaviour, 110, 244–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Milliken, G.W. (1991). Analysis of hand use in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Memphis State University, Memphis, TN.Google Scholar
  45. Milliken, G.W., Forsythe, C, & Ward, J.P. (1989). Multiple measures of hand-use lateralization in the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur cattd). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 262–268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Milliken, G.W., Stafford, D.K., Dodson, D.L., Pinger, C.D., & Ward, J.P. (1991). Analyses of feeding lateralization in the small-eared bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii): A comparison with the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 274–285.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Milliken, G.W., Ward, J.P., & Erickson, C.J. (1991). Independent digit control in foraging by the Aye-Aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Folia Primatologica, 56, 219–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Milton, K. (1978). Role of the upper canine and P2 in increasing the harvesting efficiency of Hapalemur griseus Link, 1795. Journal of Mammology, 59, 188–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Napier, J.R., & Walker, A.C. (1967). Vertical clinging and leaping: A newly recognized locomotor behavior among primates. Folia Primatologica, 6,180–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Olson, D.A., Ellis, J.E., & Nadler, R.D. (1990). Hand preferences in captive gorillas, orang-utans and gibbons. American Journal of Primatology, 20, 83–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pereira, M.E., Seeligson, M. L., & Macedonia, J.M. (1988). The behavioral repertoire of the black-and-white ruffed lemur, Varecia variegata variegata (Primates: Lemuridae). Folia Primatologica, 51, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Petersen, M.R., Beecher, M.D., Zoloth, S.R., Moody, D.B., & Stebbins, W.C. (1978). Neural lateralization of species-specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Science, 202, 324–327.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Porac, C., & Coren, S. (1981). Lateral preferences and human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  54. Ryan, A.S. (1979). Wear striation direction on primate teeth: A scanning electron microscope examination. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 50,155–168.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sanford, C., Guin, K., & Ward, J.P. (1984). Posture and laterality in the bushbaby (Galago senegalensis). Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 25, 217–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Seltzer, C., Forsythe, C, & Ward, J.P. (1990). Multiple measures of motor lateralization in human primates (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 159–166.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sherrington, C.S. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Stafford, D.K., Milliken, G.W., & Ward, J.P. (1990). Lateral bias in feeding and brachiation in Hylobates. Primates, 31, 407–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Stafford, D.K., Milliken, G.W., & Ward, J.P. (in press). Patterns of hand and mouth lateral biases in bamboo leaf shoot feeding and simple food reaching in the gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus). American Journal of Primatology.Google Scholar
  60. Subramoniam, S. (1957). Some observations on the habit of the slender loris, Loris tardigradus (Linnaeus). Journal of Bombay Natural History Society, 54, 386–398.Google Scholar
  61. Talbott, R.E. (1979). Ferner, the synergy concept, and the study of posture and movement. In R.E. Talbott & D.R. Humphrey (Eds.), Posture and movement (pp. 1–12). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  62. Tattersall, I. (1982). The primates of madagascar. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Taylor, D.C. (1969). Differential rates of cerebral maturation between sexes and between hemispheres. Lancet, 2, 140–142.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Toth, N. (1985). Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the lower and middle Pleistocene, and its possible implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 607–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Walker, A.C. (1974). A review of the Miocene Lorisidae of East Africa. In R.D. Martin, G.A. Doyle & A.C. Walker (Eds.), Prosimian behaviour (pp. 435–448). London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
  66. Ward, J.P. (1991). Prosimians as animal models in the study of neural lateralization. In F.L. Kitterle (Ed.), Cerebral laterality: Theory and research (pp. 1–17). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  67. Ward, J.P., Milliken, G.W., Dodson, D.L., Stafford, D.K., & Wallace, M. (1990). Handedness as a function of sex and age in a large population of Lemur. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 167–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wrangham, R.W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75, 262–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeannette P. Ward
  • Garrett W. Milliken
  • Donna K. Stafford

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations