Advertisement

Contextualism, Naturalistic Inquiry, and the Need for New Science: A Rethinking of Everyday Memory Aging and Childhood Sexual Abuse

  • John C. Cavanaugh
  • Kelly R. Morton

Abstract

Consider the following two events. In the first, two people embrace, get into bed, and have sexual intercourse. In the second, a person omits a step in a routine. What do these events mean? On the surface—nothing, really. But what if we added contexts, such that in the first instance it was discovered that the two people were newlyweds? Or father and daughter? Would that contextual information affect the interpretation of the event? What if the forgotten step were omitting a necessary punctuation mark in a line of computer input as opposed to how to get home from the corner? Objectively speaking, events and contexts are neutral. What an event means depends entirely on the context in which it is embedded. Intercourse between newlyweds is passionate bliss; intercourse between father and daughter is incest. Forgetting a semicolon is simply annoying; forgetting how to get home may be a symptom of Alzheimer’s disease.

Keywords

Sexual Abuse Social Context Child Abuse Childhood Sexual Abuse Cognitive Level 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alter-Reid, K., Gibbs, M.S., Lachenmeyer, J.K., Sigal, J. & Massoth, N.A. (1986). Sexual abuse of children: A review of the empirical findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 6, 249–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Argyris, C. (1980). Inner contradictions of rigorous research. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. (1988). But what the hell is it for? In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1)(pp. 3–18). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  4. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration. American Psychologist, 35, 320–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blank, T.O. (1982). A social psychology of developing adults. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  7. Bleicher, J. (1980). Contemporary hermeneutics: Hermeneutics as method, philosophy and critique. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  8. Boszormenyi-Nagy, I. (1987). Foundations of contextual therapy. New York: Brunner/ Mazel.Google Scholar
  9. Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., & Krasner, B.K. (1986). Between give and take: A clinical guide to contextual therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  10. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychology, 32, 513–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Browne, A., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abuse: A review of the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99,66–77.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Camp, C.J., & McKitrick, L.A. (1989). The dialectics of forgetting and remembering across the adult lifespan. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (Eds.), Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 169–187). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  13. Cavanaugh, J.C. (1983). Comprehension and retention of television programs by 20-and 60-year-olds. Journal of Gerontology,38, 190–196.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Cavanaugh, J.C. (1986–1987). Age differences in adults’ self-reports of memory ability: It depends on how and what you ask. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 24, 271–277.Google Scholar
  15. Cavanaugh, J.C. (1989). The importance of awareness in memory aging. In L.W. Poon, D.C. Rubin, & B. Wilson (Eds.), Everyday cognition in adult and late life. New York: Cambridge.Google Scholar
  16. Cavanaugh, J.C., Grady, J.G., & Perlmutter, M. (1983). Forgetting and use of memory aids in 20 and 70 year olds’ everyday life. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 17, 113–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cavanaugh, J.C., Kramer, D.A., Sinnott, J.D., Camp, C.J., & Markley, R.P. (1985). On missing links and such: Interfaces between cognitive research and everyday problem solving. Human Development, 28,146–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cavanaugh, J.C., & Morton, K.R. (1988). Older adults’ attributions about everyday memory. In M.M. Gruneberg, P. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and theory (Vol. 1) (pp. 209–214). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Cavanaugh, J.C., Morton, K.R., & Tilse, C.S. (1989). A self-evaluation framework for understanding everyday memory aging. In J.D. Sinnott (Ed.), Everyday problem solving: Theory and application. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  20. Cronbach, L.J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 30, 116–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dewey, J. (1908). Does reality possess practical character? In J. Dewey (Ed.), Essays, philosophical and psychological,in honor of William James (pp. 53–80). New York: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
  22. Dewey, J. (1929). Experience and nature (rev. ed.). New York: Norton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Feyerabend, P.K. (1975). Against method. London: Verso.Google Scholar
  24. Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  25. Finkelhor, D. (1986). A sourcebook on child sexual abuse. Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
  26. Garbarino, J. (1977). The human ecology of child maltreatment: A conceptual model for research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 721–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Georgoudi, M., & Rosnow, R.L. (1985). Notes toward a contextualist understandingof social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,11, 5–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gergen, K.J. (1982). Toward transformation in social knowledge. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  29. Germer, C.K. (1989). The contextual-epistemic approach to psychotherapy. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (Eds.), Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 210–233). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  30. Goodwin, J. (1982). Sexual abuse: Incest victims and their families. Boston: John Wright-PSG.Google Scholar
  31. Hahn, L.E. (1942). A contextualist theory of perception. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  32. Henderson, J. (1983). Is incest harmful? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 28, 34–39.Google Scholar
  33. Hesse, M. (1980). Revolutions and reconstructions in the philosophy of science. Bloomington: University of Indiana Press.Google Scholar
  34. Howard, R.J. (1982). Three faces of hermeneutics. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  35. Kahlbaugh, P.E. (1989). William James’ pragmatism: A clarification of the contextual world view. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (Eds.), Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 73–88). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  36. Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Knorr-Cetina, K. (1981). The manufacture of knowledge: An essay on the constructivist and contextual nature of science. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  38. Lincoln, Y.S., & Guba, E.G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverley Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
  39. Lindberg, F.H., & Distad, L.J. (1985). Survival responses to incest: Adolescents in crisis. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9,521–526.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. London, I.D. (1977). Convergent and divergent amplification and its meaning for social science. Psychological Reports,41, 111–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Margolis, J., Krausz, M., & Burian, R.M. (Eds.). (1986). Rationality, Relativism, and the human sciences. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  42. Margolis, J., Manicas, P.T., Harré, R., & Secord, P.F. (1986). Psychology: Designing the discipline, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  43. Meacham, J.A. (1989). Discovering the social-cultural context of research: Listening to and learning from research participants. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (Eds.). Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 136–153). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  44. Meehl, P.E. (1967). Theory-testing in psychology and physics: A methodological paradox. Philosophy of Science, 34,103–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mishler, E.G. (1979). Meaning in context: Is there any other kind? Harvard Educational Review, 49, 1–19.Google Scholar
  46. Mrazek, P.B., & Kempe, C.H. (1981). Sexually abused children and their families. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  47. Packer, M.J. (1985a). Hermeneutic inquiry in the study of human conduct. American Psychologist, 40, 1081–1093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Packer, M.J. (1985b). The structure of moral action: A hermeneutic study of moral conduct (Contributions to human development, Vol. 13). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
  49. Pepper, S.C. (1942). World hypotheses: A study in evidence. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  50. Pepper, S.C. (1967). Concept and quality: A world hypothesis. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
  51. Pepper, S.C. (1973). Metaphor in philosophy. In P.P. Wiener (Ed.), Dictionary of the history of ideas (pp. 196–201). New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
  52. Perlmutter, M., Kaplan, M., & Nyquist, L. (1987, August). Age differences in adults’ beliefs about aging. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York.Google Scholar
  53. Poon, L.W. (1985). Differences in human memory with aging: Nature, causes and clinical implications. In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed.) (pp. 427–462). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  54. Putnam, H. (1982). Why there isn’t a ready-made world. Synthese, 51, 141–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ratner, H.H., Schell, D.A., Cummins, A., Mittelman, D., & Baldinelli, L. (1987). Changes in adults’ prose recall: Aging or cognitive demand. Developmental Psychology, 23, 521–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rosnow, R.L. (1981). Paradigms in transition: The methodology of social inquiry. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Rosnow, R.L. (1983). Von Osten’s horse, Hamlet’s question, and the mechanistic view of causality: Implications for a post-crisis social psychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 4, 319–338.Google Scholar
  58. Rosnow, R.L., & Georgoudi, M. (1986). The spirit of contextualism. In R.L. Rosnow & M. Georgoudi (Eds.), Contextualism and understanding in behavioral science: Implications for research and theory (pp. 3–22). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  59. Scarr, S. (1985). Constructing psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 499–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Selman, R.L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: Developmental and clinical analysis. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  61. Selman, R.L., Jaquette, D., & Lavin, D.R. (1977). Interpersonal awareness in children: Toward an integration of developmental and clinical child psychology. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 47, 264–274.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Selman, R.L., Schorin, M.Z., Stone, C.R., & Phelps, E. (1983). A naturalistic study of children’s social understanding. Developmental Psychology,19, 82–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Silverman, H.J., & Ihde, D. (Eds.) (1985). Hermeneutics and deconstruction. Albany, NY: State University of New York.Google Scholar
  64. Sinnott, J.D. (1989). Changing the known; knowing the changing: The general systems theory metatheory as a conceptual framework to study complex change and complex thoughts. In D.A. Kramer & M.J. Bopp (Eds.), Transformation in clinical and developmental psychology (pp. 51–69). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  65. Smith, J.E. (1978). Purpose and thought: The meaning of pragmatism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Stake, R.E. (1978). The case-study method in social inquiry. Educational Researcher, 7, 5–8.Google Scholar
  67. Steele, B.F. (1986). Notes on the lasting effects of early child abuse throughout the life cycle. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10,283–291.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Stockman, N. (1983). Antipositivist theories of the sciences. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Reidel.Google Scholar
  69. Thayer, H.S. (1968). Meaning and action: A critical history of pragmatism. New York: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
  70. Thayer, H.S. (Ed.). (1982). Pragmatism: The classic writings. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
  71. Viney, L.L. (1987). Interpreting the interpreters. Malabar, FL: Krieger.Google Scholar
  72. Weimer, W.B. (1979). On the methodology of scientific research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  73. Weiner, B. (1985a). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Weiner, B. (1985b). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. West, R.L. (1986). Everyday memory and aging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 323–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Whitbourne, S.K. (1986). The me I know: A study of adult identity. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  77. Wolf, F.A. (1981). Taking the quantum leap. San Francisco: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  78. Wyatt, G.E., & Peters, S.D. (1986). Issues in the definition of child abuse in prevalence research. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10,231–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • John C. Cavanaugh
  • Kelly R. Morton

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations