Skip to main content

Initial Enabling Knowledge and Skills in Reading Acquisition: Print Awareness and Phonological Segmentation

  • Chapter
Phonological Awareness in Reading

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Language and Communication ((SSLAN,volume 28))

Abstract

The acquisition of reading skill does not begin with formal instruction in school. Throughout the preschool years, most children in Western societies are subjected to a great deal of informal literacy socialization. Although a majority of children enter school as nonreaders in a traditional sense, they often display surprisingly well-developed concepts of the nature and the function of written language. A skill component, however, also is involved in reading literacy, which does not easily seem to develop spontaneously in the natural ecology of a child, but which, in many cases, seems to require explicit teaching for its development. The purpose of this chapter is to discuss some of the important steps preschool children seem to take on the route to literacy and to review some empirical studies that especially reveal the critical importance of phonological awareness in reading acquisition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ayers, D., & Downing, J. (1982). Testing children’s concepts of reading. Educational Research, 24, 277–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Fischer, S. (1972). A comparison of sign language and spoken language: Rate and grammatical mechanism. Cognition, 7, 173–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P., & de Gelder, B. (1989). The emergence of phonological awareness: Comparative approaches. In I.G. Mattingly & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Modularity the motor theory of speech perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., & Goswami, U. (1987). Beyond grapheme-phoneme correspondence. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 439–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W.L. (1985). Linguistic differences produced by differences between speaking and writing. In D.R. Olson, N. Torance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning. The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 105–123 ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W.L. ( 1989, March). Where are the differences between oral and written language? Information flow in spoken and written languages. Paper presented at the American Education Research Association Annual Meeting, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, J. (1976). The reading instruction register. Language Arts, 53, 762–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (in press). Development of the ability to read words. In P.D. Pearson (Ed.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, H. (1973). Children’s experience of reading and notions of units in language. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 43, 17–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K.S., & Goodman, Y.M. (1979). Learning to read is natural. In L.B. Resnick, & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.B., & Tunmer, W.E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7 (1), 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, E. (1981). Developmental patterns and interrelationships of preschool children’s print awareness. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 236–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohn, W.E., & Ehri, L.C. (1983). Do alphabet letters help prereaders acquire phonemic segmentation skill? Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 752–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Høien, T., & Lundberg, I. (1988). Stages of word recognition in early reading development. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huba, M.E., & Kontos, S. (1985). Measuring print awareness in young children. Journal of Educational Research, 78, 272–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huba, M.E., & Robinson, S.S. ( 1987, April). Correlates of reading ability among preschoolers. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Education Research Asociation, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P.L., & Gough, P.B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kontos, S., & MacKley, H. ( 1985, April). Development and interrelationships of reading knowledge and skills during kindergarten and first grade. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Education Research Association, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J.P., Høien, T., Lundberg, I., & Ødegaard, H. (in press). MRI evaluation of the size and symmetry of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A.M., Cooper, F.S., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception and the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, B. (1989). Some remarks on the origin of the phonetic code. In C. von Euler, I. Lundberg & G. Lennerstrand (Eds.), Brain and reading (pp. 27–44 ). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1984). Sprak och lasning [Language and reading]. Malmö, Sweden: LiberFörlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1985). Longitudinal studies of reading and reading difficulties in Sweden. In G.E. MacKinnon, & T.G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4, pp. 65–105 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1987). Are letters necessary for the development of phonological awareness? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 472–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1989a). Two dimensions of decontextualization in reading acquisition. In P.B. Gough (Ed.), Theories of reading acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I. (1989b). Lack of phonological awareness. A critical factor in dyslexia. In C. von Euler, G. Lennerstrand, & I. Lundberg (Eds.), Brain and reading (pp. 221–231 ). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., & Høien, T. (in press). Patterns of information processing skills and word recognition strategies in developmental dyslexia. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O.-P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Olofsson, Ã…., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 27, 159–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience. Cognition, 24, 65–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J.M. (1984). Early reading from a developmental perspective. In D.E. Pearson (Ed.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 505–543 ). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masonheimer, P.E., Drum, P.A., & Ehri, L.C. (1984). Does environmental print identification lead children into word reading? Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 257–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattingly, I.G. (1987). Morphological structure and segmental awareness. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 488–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGee, L.M., Lomax, R.G., & Head, M.H. (1988). Young children’s written language knowledge: What environmental and functional print reading reveals. Journal of Reading Behavior, 20, 99–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J. (1988, November). Constraints on the development of phonemic aware-ness. Paper presented in the Symposium on Phonological Processes in Literacy, to honor the research of Isabelle Y. Liberman, 39th Annual Conference of the Orton Society, Tampa, Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1987). The relationships between segmental analysis and alphabetic literacy: An interactive view. Cahiers Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 415–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olofsson, A, & Lundberg, I. (1983). Can phonemic awareness be trained in kindergarten? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 35–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R. (1977). From utterance to text: The bias of language in speech and writing. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 257–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R. (1986). Learning to mean what you say: Toward a psychology of literacy. In S. deCastell, A. Luke, & K. Egan (Eds.), Literacy, society, and schooling. A reader (pp. 145–158 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., & Rack, J. (1989). Deficits in disabled readers’ phonological and orthographic coding: Etiology and remediation. In C. von Euler, I. Lundberg, & G. Lennestrand (Eds.), Brain and reading (pp. 233–242 ). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. (1986). Issues in the diagnosis and phenotype analysis of dyslexia: Implications for family studies. In S. Smith (Ed.), Genetics and learning disabilities (pp. 69–95 ). San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A., Beck, I., Bell, L.C., & Hughes, C. (1988). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. In K.E. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 39–75 ). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, D.J. (1987). A retrospective analysis of poor auditory segmenters and low achievers: Insights from three longitudinal studies of the effects of teaching sementing skills within the reading program. In M. Masland & R.L. Masland (Eds.), Preschool prevention of reading failure (pp. 121–142 ). Parkton, Md: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1988a). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–604, 612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1988b). Introduction. In K.E. Stanovich (Ed.), Children’s reading and the development of phonological awareness (pp. 7–10 ). Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich. K.E., Cunningham, A., & Feeman, D. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1982). The oral/literate continuum in discourse. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy (pp. 1–16 ). Nor-wood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taube, K. (1988). Reading acquisition and self concept. UmeÃ¥: University of UmeÃ¥.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, G.E., & McGee, L.M. (1986). Visually impaired and sighted children’s emerging concepts about written language. In D.B. Yaden, Jr., & S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning literacy (pp. 259–275 ). Portsmouth, NH: Heineman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E., Herriman, M.L., & Nesdale, A.R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 134–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren-Leubecker, A., & Carter, B.W. (1988). Reading and growth in metaling¬uistic awareness: Relation to socioeconomic status and reading readiness skills. Child Development, 59, 728–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature of and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 229–255 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lundberg, I., Høien, T. (1991). Initial Enabling Knowledge and Skills in Reading Acquisition: Print Awareness and Phonological Segmentation. In: Sawyer, D.J., Fox, B.J. (eds) Phonological Awareness in Reading. Springer Series in Language and Communication, vol 28. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3010-6_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-3010-6_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7758-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-3010-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics