Skip to main content

What Is Integrated Across Fixations?

  • Chapter
Eye Movements and Visual Cognition

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Neuropsychology ((SSNEUROPSYCHOL))

Abstract

Our strongest introspection in perception is of a stable world. However, the visual system obtains an input that is far from stable. In normal viewing conditions—when the eye is not tracking a moving object—the eyes stay relatively immobile for periods of only a fraction of a second. In between these periods of rest (called fixations), there are rapid ballistic eye movements (called saccades) in which the retinal image is merely a smear. Thus, vision usually consists of the following sequence of events: an interval of a sixth to about a half a second in which there is a stable retinal image followed by a brief interval of a smear, an interval with a different stable retinal image, another smear, and so on (Rayner, 1978a). A central question of visual perception is how the percept of a stable world emerges from all this chaos.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alpern, M. (1962). Muscular mechanisms. In H. Davson (Ed.), The Eye (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1985). The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 364–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D.A., & Rayner, K. (1983). Parafoveal visual information and semantic contextual constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 726–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., Mezzanotte, R.J., & Rabinowitz, J.C. (1982). Scene perception: detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 143–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, H. (1973). Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. Vision Research, 13, 767–782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B., & Mayer, M. (1983). Failure to integrate visual information from successive fixations. Bulletin of the Psychonomie Society, 21, 285–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, M.L., Fox, M.J., & Dick, A.O. (1973). The effect of eye movements and backward masking on perceptual location. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 110–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DenBuurman, R., Boersma, T., & Gerrisen, J.F. (1981). Eye movements and the perceptual span in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 227–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Lollo, V. (1977). On the spatio-temporal interactions of brief visual displays.In R.H. Day & G.V. Stanley (Eds.), Studies in perception (pp. 39–55) Perth: University of Western Australia Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, S.F., & Rayner, K. (1981). Contextual effects on word perception and eye movements during reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 641–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W.K., Allmeyer, D.H., & Reder, S.M. (1976). Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations. Perception & Psychophysics, 19, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J.A. (1985). Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 265–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J.M., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1987). The effects of foveal priming and extrafoveal preview on object identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 449–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J.M., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert attention and extrafoveal information use during object identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 196–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ikeda, M., & Saida, S. (1978). Span of recognition in reading. Vision Research, 18, 83–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A.W. (1989). Lexical access during eye fixations in sentence reading: Are word access codes used to integrate lexical information across interword fixations? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 444–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D.E., Brown, J.S., & Sun, J-S. (1988). Visual masking and visual integration across saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117,276–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D.E., Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1983). Evidence against visual integration across saccadic eye movements. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D.E., Zacks, J.L., & Brown, J.S. (1989). Visual memory and the perception of a stable visual environment. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 35–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Irwin, D.E., & Yantis, S. (1982). Integrating visual information from successive fixations. Science, 215, 192–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Irwin, D.E., & Yantis, S. (1983). Failure to integrate information from successive fixations. Science, 222, 188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S.D. (1987). Morphological analysis in sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 84–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, G.R., & Mackworth, N.H. (1978). Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4, 565–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, N.H. (1965). Visual noise causes tunnel vision. Psychonomie Science, 3, 67–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matin, E. (1974). Saccadic suppression: A review and an analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 899–917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., & O’Regan, J.K. (1981). Expectations increase the benefit derived from parafoveal visual information in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 634–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 578–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., & Rayner, K. (1976a). Identifying the span of the effective stimulus in reading: Literature review and theories of reading. In H. Singer & R.B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes in reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., & Rayner, K. (1976b). Asymmetry of the perceptual span in reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomie Society, 8, 365–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., Underwood, N.R., Zola, D., & Wolverton, GS. (1985). Some temporal characteristics of processing during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 168–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., & Zola, D. (1979). Is visual information integrated across successive fixations in reading? Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 221–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G.W., Zola, D., & Wolverton, G.S. (1980, April). How precise is eye guidance? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, R.E., & Rayner, K. (1981). Saccade size in reading depends upon character spaces and not visual angle. Perception & Psychophysics, 30, 395–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, W.W., & Loftus, G.R. (1980). The functional visual field during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 391–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J.K. (1981). The convenient viewing position hypothesis. In D.F. Fisher, R.A. Monty, & J.W. Senders (Eds.), Eye movements: Cognition and visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J.K., & Levy-Schoen, A. (1983). Integrating visual information from successive fixations: Does trans-saccadic fusion exist? Vision Research, 23, 765–768.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K.R., Newsome, S.L., McDonald, J.E., & Schvaneveldt, R.W. (1982). An activation-verification model for letter and word recognition: The word superiority effect. Psychological Review, 89, 573–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., & Ames, C.T. (1989). Measuring the effect of multiple eye fixations on size and shape discrimination. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science (Supplement), 30, 159

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, R.E. (1978). Picture processing during recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 284–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pirenne, M.H. (1967). Vision and the eye (2nd ed.). London: Chapman and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A.D., & Rayner K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain and Language, 14, 174–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R.K., & Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., & Balota, D.A. (1986). Inferences about eye movement control from the perceptual span in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 123–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., & Collins, W.E. (1984). Integrating pictorial information across eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 426–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., & Henderson, J.M. (1990). Role of spatial location in integration of pictorial information across saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 199–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1975). The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1978a). Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 618–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1978b). Foveal and parafoveal cues in reading. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VIL Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Balota, D.A., & Pollatsek, A. (1986). Against parafoveal semantic preprocessing during eye fixations in reading. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 473–483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Bertera, J.H. (1979). Reading without a fovea. Science, 206, 468–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Inhoff, A.W., Morrison, R., Slowiaczek, M.L., & Bertera, J.H. (1981). Masking of foveal and parafoveal vision during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 167–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., McConkie, G.W., & Ehrlich, S.F. (1978). Eye movements and integrating information across fixations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 529–544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., McConkie, G.W., & Zola, D. (1980). Integrating information across eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 206–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1983). Is visual information integrated across saccades? Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 39–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Well, A.D., & Pollatsek, A. (1980). Asymmetry of the effective visual field in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 537–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Well, A.D., Pollatsek, A., & Bertera, J.H. (1982). The availability of useful information to the right of fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 537–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saida, S., & Ikeda, M. (1979). Useful visual field size for pattern perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 119–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, A. (1977). Neuronal models for cognitive processes: Networks for learning, perception, and imagination. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 65, 141–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L.G. (1985). The corticocortical pathways for object recognition and spatial perception. In CG. Chagas (Ed.), Pattern recognition mechanisms. Rome: Pontificiae Academiae Scientiarum Scripta Varia

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L.G., & Mishkin, M. (1985). Two cortical visual systems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale, & R.J.W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A.H.C., Bridgeman, B., & Mewhort, D.J.K. (1986). Is stimulus persistence affected by eye movements? A critique of Davidson, Fox, and Dick (1973). Psychological Research, 48, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waltz, D. (1975). Understanding line drawings of scenes with shadows. In P.H. Winston (Ed.), The Psychology of computer vision. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, W., Hauske, G., & Lupp, U. (1978). How presaccadic gratings modify post-saccadic modulation transfer function. Vision Research, 18, 1173–1179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, W., Hauske, G., & Lupp, U. (1980). Interaction of pre- and post-saccadic patterns having the same coordinates in space. Vision Research, 20, 117–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R.H., Goldberg M.E., & Robinson, D.L. (1980). Behavioral modulation of visual responses in the monkey: Stimulus selection for attention and movement. In J.M. Sprague & A.N. Epstein (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 9) New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R.H., Goldberg, M.E., & Robinson, D.L. (1982). Brain mechanisms of visual attention. Scientific American, 246, 124–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag New York Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K. (1992). What Is Integrated Across Fixations?. In: Rayner, K. (eds) Eye Movements and Visual Cognition. Springer Series in Neuropsychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2852-3_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2852-3_10

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-7696-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4612-2852-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics